ebook img

Data-Book of Happiness: A Complementary Reference Work to ‘Conditions of Happiness’ by the same author PDF

580 Pages·1984·14.311 MB·English
Save to my drive
Quick download
Download
Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.

Preview Data-Book of Happiness: A Complementary Reference Work to ‘Conditions of Happiness’ by the same author

DATA-SOOK OF HAPPINESS DATA-BOOK OF HAPPINESS A Complementary Reference Work to 'Conditions of Happiness' by the same author RUUT VEENHOVEN with the assistance of TON JONKERS Erasmus University Rotterdam Department of Sociology ..... " SPRINGER-SCIENCE+BUSINESS MEDIA, B.v. Library of Congress Cataloging in Publicatlon Data Veenhoven, R. Data-Book of Happiness Bibliography: p. Includes indexes. 1. Happiness-Case Studies. i. Jonkers, Ton. li. Veenhoven, R. Conditions of Happiness. III. Title. BJ1481.v443 1984 152.4 84-9196 ISBN 978-90-277-1793-1 ISBN 978-94-015-3726-1 (eBook) DOI 10.1007/978-94-015-3726-1 AII Rights Reserved © 1984 by Springer Science+Business Media Dordrecht Originally published by D. Reidel Publishing Company, Dordrecht, Holland in 1984 No part of the material protected by this copyright notice may be reproduced or utilized in any form or by any means, electronic or mechanical, including photocopying, record ing or by any information storage and retrieval system, without written permission from the copyright owner. A second edition of this book will be published in 1988. It will cover investigations up to and including 1985. I nvestigators are therefore invited to send a copy of their reports to the author. Suggestions about relevant publications are also welcome; in particular suggestions about reports publ ished before 1975 but not covered in this volume. Please send to: [I. Ruut Veenhoven; Frasmus University Rotterdam; Department of Sociology; P.O.B. 1738,3000 DR Rotterdam, The Netherlands. TABLE OF CONTENTS ACKNOWLEDGMENT PART 1/ DESIGN 1. Purpose of the Study 5 2. The Concept of Happiness 7 3. Indicators of Happiness 11 4. Searching Empirical Happiness Studies 13 5. Presenting the Findings 21 PART II/ EXCERPTS Excerpts of 150 Reports on Empirical Investigations on Happiness between 1911 and 1975. Presented in Alphabetical Order of Author's Names 27 PART III/CORRELATES More than 3500 Correlational Findings, Ordered in 42 Main Subject Categories. Presented in Alphabetical Order 189 PART IV/PUBLIC HAPPINESS Distributions of Responses to Questions about Happiness in 121 Surveys in 32 Countries between 1940 and 1975 509 APPENDIX A Technical Terms Used in the Excerpts 525 APPENDIX B Measures of Association Used in One or More of the Studies 539 APPENDIX C Test Statistics Used in One or More of the Studies 543 REFERENCES 545 AUTHOR INDEX 562 SUBJECT INDEX 571 -1- ACKNOWLEDG MENT This book is one of the products of a research program on happiness on the Erasmus University Rotterdam. Funds were provided by the Department of Sociology of that University and also by the Dutch Ministry of Social Affairs by means of allotment of assistence. Several people assisted in excerpting reports and orderin9 the findings, among others: Peter Bakker, Rob F'euth, Hans Hordijk, Ton Jonkers, Gerrit van Kooten and Lies Kokee. Ton Jonkers, in particular, did a great deal. Technical advice was given by Lies Nuyten. The typework was done by Janet de Raad, Jeanne Hidskes and for the major part by Elly Graven. Sasqia Chin-Hon-Foei drew up the index. R.V. Erasmus University Rotterdam, The Netherlands January, 1984. PART I DESIGN 1. Purpose of the Study 2. The Concept of Happiness 3. Indicators of Happiness 4. Searching Empirical Happiness Studies 5. Presenting the Findings -5- 1. PURPOSE OF THE STUDY. Since Classical times philosophers have been fascinated by the question of how happiness can effectively and lastingly be promoted. I n that context many have wondered why all people are not equally happy, even not when I iving in identical conditions. I here is now a vast a;nount of literatur'e on the matter. However, we are little wiser. There are several reasons why differences in happiness are so little understood as yet. One is that most students of the subject have tended to con fuse moral izing and real ity, the bulk of the I iterature deal ing in fact with moral rules for living. Another reason is that speculation often predominated system atic observation. There was therefore little accumulation of knowledge. I t had been expected that the emerging social sciences would take up the empirical study of happiness and that conclusions would eventually be arrived at. Several founders of psychology and sociology saw grounds for hope: with the nai·ve optimism of their time they professed the discovery of universal laws of happiness and announced the possibility of a scientifically guided reconstruc tion of society on that basis. Yet the matter stopped with such declarations. With the exception of a few isolated attempts the subject was abandoned. Only since the 1960's has any appreciable amount of empirical investigations been performed. To some ex tent this was a by-product of the so-called 'social indicators movement'. Policymakers in affluent western nations instigated large scale surveys to as sess the well-being of citizens and to sound out the demand for state sponsored services. Several of these marketing-I ike studies involved attempts to assess the appreciation of life - in Britain for example carried out by Hall (1976); in the Netherl ands by Pommer & van Praag (1978) and in the US by Bradburn (1969), Campbe II (1976) and Andrews & Wh i they (1976). I ndependent I y of them some stray psychologists picked up the subjects as well, as did some cl inical psychologists while studying the healthy personality (among others Wessman & Ricks, 1966). A few social psychologists became interested in the issue in the course of their work on social comparison (among others Brickman & Camp bell, 1971). Several gerontologists 'discovered' happiness when studying the adjustment to retirement and old age (among others Thompson et al., 1960). This book is part of that revival. I t is in fact an account of the results yielded so far. Though the promised systematic study of happiness was never produced, stray investigations on the matter provide a quite sizable body of -6- data. If ever it comes to the redemption of this old mission, a chart of these data will be valuable. Earlier surveys. This is not the first attempt to take stock of the results of empirical investigations on happiness. In fact there are already nine litera ture surveys. Two of these deal exclusively with happiness in elderly persons tdams, 1971 ; Larson, 1978). The other seven are not restricted to special categories (Fellows, 1966; Wilson, 1967; Veenhoven, 1970; Fordyce, 1972; Robinson & Shaver, 1973; Arkoff, 1975; Nettler, 1976). All suffer from im perfections, the one by Veenhoven (first author of this book) not excluded. These imperfections are the rationale for the present study, so they deserve a short enumeration. Most surveys did not start from any clear conception of happiness. They tend to gather research reports on phenomena the investigator labeled as 'hap piness' or the like. There being various connotations in usage, a bacel of tongues is characteristically the result. Fordyce's survey contains for in stance studies on 'mood', 'life satisfaction' and 'peak experiences' (p. 19). Curiously most reviewers noted that the term 'happiness' carried different meanings, but failed to make motivated choice. Veenhoven's review did start with a formal definition of happiness, but did not use it sufficiently consistent ly in selecting the studies. Though all reviewers noted that some of the mea sures of happiness used were somewhat dubious, no one got around to sorting out the valid from the less valid. All merely reported the results whether sound or not. Only Fordyce pointed out some doubtful indicators afterwards. The earlier surveys cover only part of the investigations that were actual ly available at the time they were drawn up. The most complete review is the one by Fordyce in 1972. He claims his collection to be 'exhaustive'. Yet he covers only 18 of the 69 publications we found published prior to 1970. The flow of investigations after 1970 has not yet been reviewed at all. All surveys focus heavi lyon research in the US and overlook the considerable number of European studi es. The earlier reviewers did not enumerate all the findings actually presented in the publications covered. Only Fordyce tried to present the findings complete ly. All the others simply selected the most interesting ones from their point of view. Thus they tended to omit non-correlates and factors that did not fit in their theoretical scheme. Though inevitable in the context of short review arti cles, this practice involves a considerable loss of information. -7- Parallelbook 'Conditions of Happiness'. This volume is an inventory of facts. I t does not go into their interpretation. The consequences of the various findings are considered in a simultaneously published book, titled 'Conditions of Happiness', for which the present volume served in fact as a source (Veen hoven, 1984). The introductory chapters of that book provide more detai I about this study, in particular about the conceptual delineation of happiness and the problems of measurement. Hence these subjects will be mentioned only shortly in the next two sections of this chapter. 2. THE CONCEPT OF HAPPINESS. The term 'happiness' has various subtly different meanings. Its many connota tions have often proved confusing, thereby hindering the scientific study of happiness to a great extent. Thus a first step is to decide on a clear defini tion. a. Overall happiness. The term 'happiness' is used to refer to an experiental phenomenon. Overall happiness is defined as the degree to which an individual judges the overall qual ity of his I ife favorably. I n other words: how well he I ikes the I ife he leads. The key terms in this definition may be elucidated as follows: Degree. The word 'happiness' does not denote an optimal appreciation of life. In this language it depicts a degree, like the concepts of 'length' or 'weight'; it denotes more or less of something. When saying a person i2..happy, it is meant he/she judges his or her life favorably rather than unfavorably. I ndividual. The term happiness is used to describe the state of an individual person only. The term does not apply to collectivities, objects or events. So a nation cannot be said to be happy. At best, a majority of its citizens consi ders itself happy. Happiness denotes a subjective appreciation of life by an individual. So there is no given standard for happiness. While a person wr.o thinks he has a heart condition mayor may not have one, a person who thinks he is (un)happy really is (un)happy. Judges. The word 'happiness' is used where somebody made an overall judg ment about the quality of his life. This implies an intellectual activity. Making an overall judgment impl ies assessing past experiences and estimating future experiences. Both require marshalling facts into a convenient number of cog nitive categories. I t also requires awarding relative values and setting priori ties. Thus happiness is nora simple sum of pleasures, but rather a congnitive construction which the individual puts together from his various experiences.

See more

The list of books you might like

Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.