Craniofacial Anomalies Rebecca A. Eder Editor Craniofacial Anomalies Psychological Perspectives With a Foreword by Jeffrey L. Marsh, M.D. With 30 Illustrations Springer-Verlag New York Berlin Heidelberg London Paris Tokyo Hong Kong Barcelona Budapest Rebecca A. Eder Department of Psychology St. Louis Children's Hospital St. Louis, MO 63110, USA and Department of Psychology Washington University St. Louis, MO 63130, USA Library of Congress Cataloging in Publication Data Eder, Rebecca A. Craniofacial anomalies: psychological perspectives/Rebecca A. Eder. p. cm. Includes bibliographical references and index. ISBN-13: 978-1-4612-7549-7 e-ISBN-13: 978-1-4612-2466-2 DOl: 10.1007/978-1-4612-2466-2 1. Face-Abnormalities. 2. Skull-Abnormalities. I. Title. QM695.F32E34 1995 617.5'2043'019-dc20 93-31776 Printed on acid-free paper. © 1995 Springer-Verlag New York, Inc. Softcover reprint of the hardcover 1st edition 1995 All rights reserved. This work may not be translated in whole or in part without the written permission of the publisher (Springer-Verlag New York, Inc., 175 Fifth Avenue, New York, NY 10010, USA), except for brief excerpts in connection with reviews or scholarly analysis. Use in connection with any form of information storage and retrieval, electronic adaptation, computer software, or by similar or dissimilar methodology now known or hereafter developed is forbidden. The use of general descriptive names, trade names, trademarks, etc., in this publication, even if the former are not especially identified, is not to be taken as a sign that such names, as understood by the Trade Marks and Merchandise Marks Act, may accordingly be used freely by anyone. While the advice and information in this book are believed to be true and accurate at the date of going to press, neither the authors nor the editors nor the publisher can accept any legal responsibility for any errors or omissions that may be made. The publisher makes no warranty, express or implied, with respect to the material contained herein. Production coordinated by Chernow Editorial Services and managed by Terry Kornak; manufacturing supervised by Jacqui Ashri. Typeset by Best-set Typesetter Ltd., Hong Kong. 9 8 7 6 5 432 1 To Barbara Foreword The motivation for the first surgical reconstruction of a congenital facial deformity is unknown. Ancient medical writings and Renaissance woodcuts document attempts to repair cleft lip, the most frequently encountered and necessarily recognized of the craniofacial anomalies. It is probable that restoration of a "normal" appearance to destigmatize the affected individual was at least one, if not the only, objective of surgical inter vention. While the psychosocial consequences of craniofacial deformity were and are appreciated by cultures less medically sophisticated than ours, the physiological consequences of craniofacial anomalies have only been defined over the past century. Emphasis on physiological rather than psychosocial function tended to dominate the medical literature on cranio facial anomalies from the mid-nineteenth to the mid-twentieth centuries. This probably reflects both the development and popularization of sur gery based on pathophysiology and the paucity of tools and investigators for psychosocial research during that time period. The introduction of general anesthesia, antisepsis, diagnostic medical imaging, antibiotics, intravenous fluids, blood component therapy, and intensive care and specialized surgical facilities, along with the reorganization of medical education, have allowed surgery to be extended to every locus in the body no matter how remote or seemingly dangerous. While operations upon the face and skull allowed individuals affected with craniofacial anomalies to breathe, eat, see, hear, and speak more normally, normal psychosocial development and social integration did not necessarily follow. By the 1960s, some surgeons and pediatricians were questioning the efficacy of certain operations for craniosynostosis, that is, premature fusion of cranial sutures. It had been thought, for almost one hundred years, that fused sutures needed to be surgically "released" to prevent increased intracranial pressure, hydrocephalus, blindness, mental retardation, and even death. On critical examination, there was minimal evidence to support these assumptions. Reconstruction of craniofacial anomalies to facilitate the affected individual's psychosocial development and social integration had become an accepted indication for surgery by the 1970s. Normalization of psychosocial development by normalization of cranio facial appearance has remained both an important indication for and a major objective of surgery over the past quarter century. Although it seems intuitively obvious that facial deformity negatively affects psychosocial development and that, conversely, surgical recon- vii viii Foreword struction of the deformity will positively affect psychosocial development, the validity of these assumptions is yet to be proven. Surgeons not only accept "psychosocial benefit" as an indication for an operation but also use similar reasoning to advise surgery prior to the individual's becoming aware of the implications of facial deformity, even if additional surgery will be necessary as the child grows. It is assumed that the psycho logical morbidity of repetitive operations is less than that of unrecon structed facial deformity throughout childhood followed by one operation upon maturity, provided that .t he surgery effectively destigmatizes the deformity. What is the evidence to support these assumptions and medical behavior? In this text, Eder has edited an outstanding collection of contributions from leading authorities on craniofacial surgery and psychosocial development in an attempt to document current under standing and, more importantly, to provide stimulation and guidance for necessary further research. Part 1 addresses appearance, expression, and perception. Munro provides a craniofacial surgeon's perspective regarding the methods and rationale of reconstruction of craniofacial anomalies. He ques tions: Whether craniofacial surgery avoids or diminishes psychosocial problems? Whether multiple surgeries are preferable to one definitive surgery with respect to psychosocial benefit? and How are the bio logical versus psychological needs of an individual with a craniofacial anomaly best met? His observation, that although the postoperative appearance may be less disfiguring than that preoperatively it often is not normal, raises the question of how much deviation from the norm is compatible with normal psychosocial acceptance. Langlois addresses three types of information which the facial appearance communicates that regulate social behavior and interaction: age, gender, and affect. She postulates that individuals with craniofacial anomalies appear older than their chronological age or of ambiguous age and gender. This confusion may yield misperception by others that results in inappropriate expectations about and reactions toward the affected individual. Berry introduces the concept of the event perception approach to facial percep tion. This approach recognizes that in addition to nondynamic structural characteristics, such as the anthropometric measurements recorded for facial harmonics, the dynamic characteristics of motion (e.g., emotion) and change (e.g., growth) affect facial perception. If alteration of facial form, that is, nondynamic characteristics, is achieved at the ex pense of diminution of expressive motion, one must ask whether the alteration is cost/benefit effective. Shepard and Magai note that socio emotional development contains both intra- and interpersonal aspects. They discuss the important role of caregivers in the socialization of emotional expression and in the affective, experiential development Foreword ix of their children. Furthermore in the context of infant-mother inter action, there is a two-way street between infant and mother facial ex pressivity. They question how the mother of an infant with limited or idiosyncratic facial display learns to interpret that infant's emotions and needs. Attachment and parenting is the subject of Part 2. Field notes that a preference for physical attractiveness produces differential expec tations based on that physical attractiveness. These, in turn, may yield differential behavior that is attractiveness driven. Unattractive children have been observed to be less competent in social development than physically attractive peers. With respect to infants with craniofacial anomalies, they seem to be less active and less responsive than normal infants. Similarly, their mothers seem less active and responsive than those of the unaffected infants. The morbidity of these unresponsive early interactions is unknown. Petersen, Pawl, and Lieberman further discuss attachment in infants with disabilities. They note that the maternal infant relation tends to focus on negative rather than positive aspects for infants with craniofacial anomalies. The primary focus of studies of maternal-infant relations changes from the infant in studies of normals to the mother in studies of abnormal infants. Nonetheless no association between parental responsiveness and the degree of disability in the child has been shown: different forms and degrees of parental responsiveness are the norm. Attachment in infants and preschoolers who require hospitalization is discussed by Crowell, Waters, and Szanjnberg. They note that attachment can be formalized into the child's ability to use the parent as a base for exploration and a safe haven, as well as the child's confidence in parental availability and responsiveness. Parents need to be "present, predictable, and confident." Health care professionals should evaluate and use the "parents' potential as collaborators in treatment." Eder introduces Part 3, which focuses on social cognition, self-concept, and social interaction. She explores the emotional basis of self-under standing to better understand development of the self-concepts of children with cleft lip/palate. Examination of the literature presents contradictory data as to whether being born with a cleft lip/palate results in adjustment problems. Resolution of this inconsistency may reside in considering the deformity in the context of the individual and family life cycles. Eder postulates that the child's emotionality develops out of the interaction between infant temperament, parental personality, and the caregiver infant attachment. The effect or lack thereof of the cleft lip/palate on the affected individual's emotionality and subsequent self-concept may relate to whether or not one or more of these three variables is impacted as a consequence of the cleft. In a pilot study of preschoolers with clefts, she observed that both the affected children and their mothers rated them x Foreword higher on feelings of aggression and stress than their peers or their peers' mothers. Rubin and Wilkinson explore peer rejection and social isolation in childhood. They note that peer interaction is necessary for normal social and emotional development. The pathway to rejection and isolation may begin with the parent-infant interaction: if the parent withdraws from the deformed infant, the infant will be stimulated less; the infant then may respond less and in turn induce less response from the parent, thereby perpetuating a vicious cycle that results in an insecure attachment relationship. In older children, the peer response to craniofacial anomalies may be bimodal: isolation may result from being ignored or excessive attention may be drawn through teasing. Since the behavioral correlates of unattractiveness and facial deformity seem to differ, research on unattractiveness may not be relevant for craniofacial anomalies. Reis and Hodgins conclude the discussions with concentration on the physical attractiveness and stigma literatures. They raise the question of whether beauty is a more positive attribute or ugliness a more negative one. They note that the research on unattractiveness focuses on the effects of physical attractiveness rather than those of unattractiveness. The impact of stigmatizing deformity can be evaluated with respect to concealability, course over time, disruptiveness, aesthetic factors, origin, and peril posed to others. While unattractiveness seems to induce only negative outcomes, facial disfigurement can have advantages as well as dis advantages for the affected individual. Others tend to both help and hinder stigmatized individuals to a degree greater than that for non stigmatized persons. In Part 4, authors Serofica, Tobiasen, Speltz, Galbreath, and Greenberg discuss investigations into the psychosocial consequences of uncorrected or incompletely corrected craniofacial deformity. Studies on this and of the timing of such reconstructive surgery, and of effective reconstruction are necessary to both validate, hopefully, the above-stated hypotheses, upon which current management is predicated, and to better understand the general issue of appearance and psychosocial function. Dr. Eder and her colleagues report such investigations in this text. While more ques tions are asked than answered, an excellent review of relevant attractive ness, caregiver-infant bonding, socioemotional development, event perception, and emotionality literature is provided for those unfamiliar with it. General aspects of psychosocial development and deviations are focused on data from studies of individuals with craniofacial anomalies when such studies exist. More importantly, in my opinion, are the recommendations of the various contributors for directions of further research. Enough time has elapsed since the popularization of major craniofacial reconstructive surgery that infants who received such opera tions have grown into adults. The lack of adequate prospective studies has limited the information that can be recovered from the experiences of these individuals. The challenge to readers of this book is to design such Foreword Xl studies so that the infants and young children who are operated on in the near future may meaningfully contribute to our understanding of facial deformity and its reconstruction in the not too distant morrow. Jeffrey L. Marsh, M.D. Professor of Surgery, Plastic and Reconstructive Associate Professor of Pediatrics in Surgery (Plastic and Reconstructive) Professor of Radiology, Division of Radiology Research Washington University School of Medicine and Medical Director Cleft Palate and Craniofacial Deformities Institute St. Louis Children's Hospital St. Louis, MO 63110 Preface Craniofacial anomalies (CFAs) represent one of the most commonly occurring birth defects. Clefts of the lip and/or palate (CLP) alone occur in approximately 1.5 out of every 1,000 births. The prevalence of all the other anomalies is equal to or exceeds that of CLP (Day, 1985). Recent diagnostic and surgical advances have resulted in substantial changes in the medical treatment of persons with craniofacial anomalies. However, relatively little has changed in the approach to psychological issues associated with CFA s. The prevailing assumption on the part of clinicians and researchers-that facial disfigurement must result in psychological problems-has remained unchallenged for nearly half a century (e.g., Macgregor, Abel, Byrt, Lauer, & Weissman, 1953; Pruzinsky, 1990). This book was undertaken to advance our understanding of the psychological impact of CFA s by reevaluating the prevailing view in light of the most current perspectives and findings in developmental psychology. In a review of craniofacial birth defects, Day stated that, "Without regard to its severity or rarity, each CFA takes on additional significance because it affects the human face. No other portion of our body is invested with so much meaning, recognition, and importance" (1985, p. 344). Reviews such as these often describe specific psychological problems believed to be associated with these anomalies. For example, CFAs have been speculated to be associated with problems in mother-infant bonding, self-esteem, social acceptance, school performance and adjustment, and adult mental heath (e.g., Day, 1985; Kapp-Simon, 1981; Pruzinsky, 1990). There is a considerable discrepancy between the perceived impact of CFA s on psychological functioning and conclusions derived from actual empirical research. For example, persons with CFAs have not been found to differ from the norm on measures of psychopathology (e.g., Richman & Eliason, 1982; Williams, 1982). Furthermore, few reliable differences are reported on measures of self-esteem and self-perception (e.g., Eder, this volume). What accounts for this discrepancy? Previous investigators have attributed the preponderance of non significant results to the use of unreliable measures and/or to the lack of appropriate control groups with which to compare individuals with CFA s (e.g., Clifford, 1987; Tobiasen, 1984). For example, few of these studies have systematically varied or examined the effect of degree of disfigure- xiii