Corruption, Political Allegiances, and Attitudes Toward Government in Contemporary Democracies Christopher J. Anderson Binghamton University, SUNY Yuliya V. Tverdova Binghamton University, SUNY Usingsurveysconductedinsixteenmatureandnewlyestablisheddemocraciesaroundtheglobe,thisstudyexaminesthe effectofcorruptiononpeople’sattitudestowardgovernment.Theanalysisdemonstratesthatcitizensincountrieswithhigher levelsofcorruptionexpressmorenegativeevaluationsoftheperformanceofthepoliticalsystemandexhibitlowerlevelsof trustincivilservants.However,theresultsalsoshowthatthenegativeeffectofcorruptiononevaluationsofthepolitical systemissignificantlyattenuatedamongsupportersoftheincumbentpoliticalauthorities.Thesefindingsprovidestrong andsystematicevidencethatinformalpoliticalpractices,especiallythosethatcompromiseimportantdemocraticprinciples, shouldbeconsideredimportantindicatorsofpoliticalsystemperformance.Moreover,theyimplythat,whilecorruption isapowerfuldeterminantofpoliticalsupportacrosswidelyvaryingpolitical,cultural,andeconomiccontexts,itdoesnot uniformlydiminishsupportforpoliticalinstitutionsacrossallsegmentsoftheelectorate. T heprinciplesunderlyingdemocraticpoliticalsys- thatthenegativeeffectof corruptiononpeople’sbeliefs tems presume that governments are accountable aboutgovernmentisfilteredthroughvoters’politicalal- to their citizens, that they administer laws equi- legiance. Building on the insight that those who voted tably and fairly, that their actions are transparent, and for the incumbent government are more likely to eval- that all citizens have access to the political process. As uate the performance of political institutions positively, a result, political scientists have long assumed that po- wefindthatcorruptionhaslessofacorrodingeffecton litical systems that fail to live up to these promises are people’sevaluationsofthepoliticalsystem’sperformance likely be plagued with low levels of legitimacy. This amongsupportersofthegovernmentthanamongthose study investigates one feature of modern democracy— whoopposeit. corruption—thatsystematicallyunderminesdemocratic Thisarticleseekstomakeseveralcontributions.First, principles and, as a result, diminishes people’s faith in it aims to enhance our knowledge of the distortion ef- thepoliticalprocess.Wearguethatcorruptionisanim- fectsproducedbycorruption.Eventhoughcorruptionis portant indicator of the performance of a political sys- widelyassumedtohavenegativeconsequencesforacoun- tem and show that high levels of corruption reduce cit- try’ssocial,economic,andpoliticallife,politicalscientists izen support for democratic political institutions across havenotsystematicallyexaminedhowitaffectspeople’s mature and newly established democracies around the views of the political system and institutions of govern- globe. Moreover, we hypothesize and show empirically ment. Instead, political researchers have focused mainly ChristopherJ.AndersonisProfessorandChair,DepartmentofPoliticalScience,Fellow,CenteronDemocraticPerformance,Bingham- tonUniversity,SUNY,Binghamton,NewYork13902-6000([email protected]).YuliyaV.TverdovaisaPh.D.Candidateinthe DepartmentofPoliticalScience,BinghamtonUniversity,SUNY,Binghamton,NewYork13902-6000([email protected]). Earlierversionsofthisarticlewerepresentedatthe2000AnnualMeetingoftheMidwestPoliticalScienceAssociationandtheAnnual MeetingoftheAmericanPoliticalScienceAssociation,Washington,D.C.WearedeeplyindebtedtoMichaelD.McDonaldforallhishelpin revisingthisarticle.WealsowouldliketothankseminarparticipantsattheUniversityofOxford(NuffieldCollege)andCornellUniversity (DepartmentofGovernmentandJohnsonGraduateSchoolofManagement)fortheirinsightfulcomments.ThanksalsotoRayDuch, Hans-DieterKlingemann,andtheanonymousreviewersfortheirthoughtfulcommentsonearlierdrafts,SilviaMendesforhelpwith thedata,andMarcoSteenbergen,HarveyPalmer,andChrisZornforadviceregardingtheanalyses.Asalways,manythankstoKathleen O’Connorforallherhelp.ThisresearchwassupportedbyNSFgrantSES-9818525toChrisAnderson.Thesurveydataareavailableas ICPSRStudyNo.2808.Theoriginalcollectorofthedata,ICPSR,andtherelevantfundingagencybearnoresponsibilityforusesofthis collectionorforinterpretationsorinferencesbaseduponsuchuses.ThedatawereanalyzedwiththeMLwiN,Version1.10,statistical software. AmericanJournalofPoliticalScience,Vol.47,No.1,January2003,Pp.91–109 (cid:2)C2003bytheMidwestPoliticalScienceAssociation ISSN0092-5853 91 92 CHRISTOPHERJ.ANDERSONANDYULIYAV.TVERDOVA ontheeffectsofcorruptionondemocraticprinciplessuch systemperformancethatshapesthereputationofpoliti- as accountability, equality, and fairness. We go beyond calinstitutions.Incontrast,thequestionofhowpolitical theseearlierstudiesbyestablishingaconnectionbetween performance affects system support has received much corruptionandmassperceptionsofhowwellthepolitical morelimitedattentionbysocialscientists.1Thefewstud- systemworksandwhetherpublicofficialscanbetrusted ies that do exist are important, however, because they across widely different contemporary democracies. Sec- show that political performance and the functioning of ond,ourresearchbreaksnewgroundinthestudyofpo- formalpoliticalinstitutionsmattersforhowpeopleview liticallegitimacybyfocusingoncorruption,anindicator thefunctioningofthepoliticalsystem.Below,weseekto ofpoliticalperformanceandanexplanatoryvariablenot add to our understanding of the effects of political per- usually examined in studies of political support. Thus, formance on political support by investigating whether we seek to develop a more complete understanding of andhowinformalinstitutionsandpracticesintheform thelegitimacyofpoliticalsystemsbyexaminingwhether ofcorruptioninfluencepeople’sattitudestowardtheex- andhowcorruptionaffectsattitudesaboutgovernment. istingpoliticalorder. Third, our research examines the contingent nature of Examining the effects of corruption, which we de- therelationshipsamongcorruption,politicalallegiances, fine as “the misuse of public office for private gain” andpoliticalsupport.Inparticular,weseektodetermine (SandholtzandKoetzle2000,32),meansinvestigatinga whetherindividual-levelfactors,suchashavingallegiance phenomenonwhoseexistenceismoredifficulttoconcep- to those in power, can serve to neutralize the negative tualizeandmeasurethanthatofeconomicperformance impact of corruption on people’s views of the political orformalpoliticalinstitutions,andwhoseconsequences system. If they do, inferior performance may not nec- arenotalwaysobvious.Avoluminousliteraturehasdoc- essarily be recognized by all citizens, and we may not umentedthenegativeeffectsofcorruptiononanation’s see a decrease in the level of public support for politi- socialandeconomiclife(forreviews,seeMontinolaand calauthorityasaresultamongimportantsegmentsofthe Jackman2002;Rose-Ackerman1999).Moreimportantly electorate. for the purposes of this study, however, corruption also Thenextsectiondescribessomeofthegapsinwhat hasbeenfoundtofundamentallyunderminetheprinci- weknowabouttherelationshipbetweencorruptionand plesofdemocraticaccountability,equality,andopenness mass support for the political system. Subsequently, we (Dahl 1971).2 When corruption is present, democracy’s discussthehypothesizedeffectof politicalallegianceon tenets of procedural and distributive fairness become a attitudesaboutgovernment,aswellasourcontentionof myth; this, in turn, is likely to diminish the legitimacy why the effect of corruption on political support may hinge, in part, on membership in the political majority 1Fewcomparativestudieshaveexaminedhowformalpoliticalin- and minority. We then turn to issues of measurement stitutionsandtheiroutputsaffectsupportforthepoliticalsystem. anddataanalysis.Afterpresentingtheresults,wediscuss Amongthem,MillerandListhaug(1990,1999),forexample,have the importance of the findings for the study of political foundthatopportunitiestoexpressdiscontentandpositiveper- ceptionsofproceduralandoutcomefairnessarerelatedtopositive supportindemocraciesandspelloutavenuesforfurther attitudesaboutgovernment(seealsoHofferbertandKlingemann research. 1999).Similarly,studieshaveshownthatmoreproportionalelec- toralsystemsareassociatedwithhigherlevelsof regimesupport (Anderson1998).Asidefrominstitutionalelementssuchasoppor- CorruptionandAttitudes tunitiesfordissent,itappearsthatgovernmentstabilitymattersfor howpeopleviewthepoliticalsystem.Specifically,peopleinsystems TowardGovernment withmoredurablegovernmentsaremoresupportiveoftheexist- ingpoliticalarrangements(HarmelandRobertson1986).Finally, studiesofsystemsupportinnewdemocracieshavepointedtothe Political trust or system support is an important indi- importanceofpoliticalperformanceasdeterminantsofsupportfor catorof ahealthycivicanddemocraticpoliticalculture. democraticinstitutionsmoregenerally(EvansandWhitefield1995; Scholarscommonlyassumethatdisenchantedcitizensare MishlerandRose1997,2001a),andthattheinstitutionalqualityof domesticinstitutionscanaffectsupportforsupranationalinstitu- morelikelytopushforradicalchangesinthesystem,and tions(Rohrschneider2002). that distrust of government may be detrimental to the 2Forexample,corruptionunderminesdemocraticrulewhenpublic establishmentandsurvivalofdemocraticlifeinthelong goodsareavailableonlyforthosewhohaveeitherconnectionsor run.Italsoiswidelyacknowledgedthatsystemoutputs— money(orboth)(Treisman2000).Andalthoughcorruptiondoes also commonly referred to as system performance—are notnecessarilypreventagovernmentfromaccomplishingsociety’s key to understanding why public support for the politi- ends,itwilldosoinefficiently.Asaresult,corruptionviolatesim- portantprinciplesofmodernbureaucracy,includingtheideathat calsystemfluctuates(Easton1965).Curiously,economic publicagenciesshouldoperateinanimpartialandrule-basedfash- performance has been the most widely studied facet of ion(SandholtzandKoetzle2000). CORRUPTIONANDATTITUDESTOWARDGOVERNMENT 93 ofdemocraticpoliticalinstitutions(GibsonandCaldeira levelsofsupportfortheregimeandadecreasedlikelihood 1995;Tyler1990). thatpeoplewouldrejectundemocraticalternatives.Sim- The idea that corruption has a negative impact on ilarly,MishlerandRose’s(2001a)studyofpoliticaltrust people’sviewsoftheirgovernmentisopentoboththeo- across10East-CentralEuropeanstatesfoundthathigher reticalandempiricalchallenge,however.Onatheoretical levelsofcorruptionwererelatedtolowerlevelsofpolitical level,somewouldarguethatacountry’spoliticalculture, trust.However,bothstudiesalsoreportedthat,onceal- which shapes people’s perception of corruption, can di- ternativeexplanationsofsystemsupportweretakeninto minishoreliminatetherelationshipwehypothesizehere. account, the effect of corruption as a predictor of sys- Culture provides a lens for how people view the world, tem support was substantially attenuated or reduced to motives for human behavior, criteria for evaluating ac- insignificance (Rose, Mishler, and Haerpfer 1998, 189– tions, and, more generally, orientations to action, all of 94;MishlerandRose2001a).Anindividual-levelstudyof whicharelearnedduringculturalsocialization(Eckstein Bolivia,Nicaragua,Paraguay,andElSalvadorshowedthat 1988).If acountry’sculturalcontextpredisposespeople people’sexperienceswithcorruptionarenegativelycorre- to view corruption as acceptable practice and therefore latedwithdiffuseregimesupport(Seligson2002).Given relatively benign, measures of corruption may not co- thepotentialforendogeneityamongtheprimaryvariables incide with how people in different cultural settings re- of interest (trust and reports of corruption) inherent in spondtocorruptpoliticalpractices(cf.Nye1967).Under the study’s cross-sectional individual-level research de- theseconditions,higherlevelsof corruptionshouldnot signandtheselect,smallnumberofcountriesexamined, be associated with more negative attitudes toward gov- these results are suggestive of a correlation among the ernment. This view also is consistent with a strand of relevantindividual-levelconstructsbutfarfromgeneral- research labeled “the functional theory” after Merton’s izableacrossawiderrangeofcontemporarydemocracies. (1968)seminaldiscussionof thelatentfunctionsof po- Atthistime,theempiricalrecordthusisinsufficient liticalmachines,whichhighlightsthepositiveaspectsas- and open to crucial challenges when it comes to deter- sociated with the buying and selling of political favors mining conclusively whether there is a relationship be- (Leff 1964; Huntington 1968; see also Montinola and tween corruption and system support at the individual Jackman2002).Viewedfromthisperspective,corruption leveloncealternativeexplanationsofsystemsupportare mayincreasetheefficiencyofgovernmentbecausepublic accountedfor.Forone,whenpresent,theeffectsofcor- servants may become more helpful and effective if paid ruption on system support have not been found to be directly. Thus, bribes may help overcome bureaucratic overwhelminglystrong.Moreover,becausethesampleof obstacles and “add to a nation’s economic efficiency” countriesconsideredinthesestudieshasbeenlimitedto (Goldsmith1999,869). newdemocraciesinEasternEuropeorLatinAmerica— Empirically,thehypothesisthatthereisarelationship thatis,countriesthatscorerelativelyhighoncorruption betweencorruptionandattitudestowardgovernmentis and related factors, such as level of development—it is also open to challenge, given the limited evidence the uncertainwhethercorruptioniscorrelatedwithpolitical nascentliteratureonthetopichasproduced.Simplyput, supportincountriesrepresentingvaryinglevelsofwealth todateresearchershavepaidlittlesystematicattentionto anddifferentpoliticalcultures. theeffectscorruptionmighthaveontheattitudesofor- Moreover,itisyettobedeterminedwhethercitizens dinarypeopletowardpoliticalinstitutionsintheircoun- associatecorruptionwiththepoliticalsystemgenerallyor try.Thisissurprising,giventhatscholarshavepointedto specificpoliticalactors,suchasgovernmentbureaucrats. attitudesaboutgovernmentresponsiveness(externalef- Below,wethereforecomparetheeffectofcorruptionon ficacy)andproceduralfairnessasimportantindicatorsof onegeneralattitudetowardgovernment—evaluationsof supportforthepoliticalsystem(Iyengar1980;Millerand the performance of the political system generally—and Listhaug1999).Andwhilethereareafewcasestudiesthat onespecificattitudeparticularlyrelevanttothequestion discuss the relationship between corruption and system of corruption—trustincivilservants.Attheoutset,itis legitimacy(e.g.,dellaPorta2000;Pharr2000),fewschol- not obvious whether both attitudes should be affected, arshaveexaminedtherelationshipsystematicallyfroma orwhethertheyshouldbeaffectedsimilarlybythepres- cross-nationalperspective. ence of corruption. Given that much of everyday cor- Among the few studies that systematically exam- ruptionisperpetratedbygovernmentbureaucrats,while ine the impact of corruption on system support, Rose, the political system is a more abstract and distant ob- Mishler, and Haerpfer’s (1998) cross-national study of ject of consideration, attitudes toward civil servants are nine Central and East European countries found that themoreproximatemeasureinthecontextofthisstudy. higher levels of corruption were associated with lower Followingthislogic,wewouldexpectstrongereffectsof 94 CHRISTOPHERJ.ANDERSONANDYULIYAV.TVERDOVA corruptionontrustincivilservantsthanonevaluations ruptionlessnegativelythanthoseintheminority.Ifthis of system performance. At the same time, it is plausi- is the case, the effect of corruption on attitudes toward ble that people will be more willing to identify corrup- government should be smaller for the political majority tion as a systemic problem than to lay the blame for thanthepoliticalminority. corruptiononthedoorstepofspecificactorstheyhaveen- Ourexpectationsregardingthisinteractionarebased countered.Afterall,itispossiblethatcitizensthemselves onresearchshowingthatpeoplearepronetoviewthepo- either are civil servants or know someone who is and, liticalworldinwaysthatareconsistentwiththeirpoliti- therefore,maybemorewillingtoblameananonymous calpredispositions(Zaller1992).Publicopinionscholars set of institutions than specific individuals they know. theorizethatindividualsuseheuristicstofilter,andtosim- Moreover,becausethepoliticalprocessinvolvesmultiple plify analysis of, the information they choose to receive governmental agents, citizens may have more evidence (Sniderman,Brody,andTetlock1991;Zaller1992).Wear- availableaboutthesystemandhowwellitfunctionsthan guethat,inthecontextofanalyzingcorruptionandsup- about specific actors (Weatherford 1987). Whether cor- portforthepoliticalsystem,therelevantheuristicislikely ruption is viewed as a systemic phenomenon, as result- tobesupportfortheincumbentpoliticalauthorities.Em- ing from the actions of a specific group of political ac- pirically,thisexpectationis,generallyspeaking,compati- tors, or both, is therefore a question we seek to answer blewiththenotionthatindividualswithparticularviews below. interpretnewinformationsothatitreinforcespreviously held attitudes, thereby augmenting rather than temper- ingthedifferencesbetweentheirbeliefsandthoseofin- TheContingentEffectsofCorruption dividualswithopposingpredispositions(Zaller1992).It onPoliticalSupport:TheRole is also consistent with the finding that people exagger- ofPoliticalAllegiances ate the performance of the macroeconomy in line with their partisan leanings, for example (Duch, Palmer, and We also posit that the effects of corruption on attitudes Anderson2000).Finally,itisconsistentwithMishlerand toward government are strengthened or weakened, de- Rose’sconjecturethattheeffectsofcorruptionarelikely pending on whether people have a stake in the existing tobemediatedbymicro-levelattitudes(MishlerandRose political regime and the maintenance of the status quo. 2001a,50). Thisexpectationemergesfromagrowingnumberofstud- Aside from the psychological mechanisms that may ies,whichshowthatcitizenswhoidentifywithorvoted condition people’s views of corruption, there also may foragoverningpartyarepredisposedtoevaluatethegov- be direct pay-offs associated with corruption that are ernment’s performance positively and to be more sup- more likely to accrue to one part of the population portive of the political system (Anderson and Guillory thananother.Inparticular,wehypothesizethatgovern- 1997; Anderson and LoTempio 2002; Ginsberg and ment supporters are more likely to be the beneficiaries Weissberg 1978; Nadeau and Blais 1993; Norris 1999).3 of the goods distributed by corrupt public officials in Wetesttwokeyhypothesesassociatedwiththisliterature. the form of attention by officials, favorable treatment First,wehypothesizethatindividualswhobelongtothe in the awarding of government contracts, or patron- political majority are more likely to exhibit positive at- age jobs, for example (Fiorina and Noll 1978; Golden titudes toward government than those in the minority. n.d.;Warner1997).Asaresult,thosewithanallegiance Second,andmoreimportant,wearguethatpeople’spo- to the incumbent government also are likely to take a litical allegiances motivate them to connect corruption morebenignviewofcorruptionbecauseitbenefitsthem withtheirviewsofthepoliticalsystemindifferentways. personally. Specifically,wepositthatpoliticalmajorityandminority In sum, we argue that those who elected the in- status acts as a screen for how people view government cumbent government are less likely to seek out infor- corruption and the extent to which they use corruption mation about corruption and less likely to interpret to judge the performance of the political system. Those such information negatively. Moreover, we posit that, whoelectedthecurrentgovernmentarelikelytoviewcor- whencorruptionoccurs,itismorelikelytobenefitsup- porters of the government than supporters of the op- 3The so-called “winner effect” or “home-team effect” has been position. As a result, corruption should produce less documentedwithregardtoanumberofdifferentpoliticalattitudes of a negative impact on political support among those such as feelings of government responsiveness, satisfaction with who elected the incumbent government (the majority) democracy, as well as people’s willingness to engage in political than among those who supported the opposition (the activism(cf.AndersonandTverdova2001;ClarkeandAcock1989; GinsbergandWeissberg1978;WhiteleyandSeyd1998). minority). CORRUPTIONANDATTITUDESTOWARDGOVERNMENT 95 DataandMeasures Togaugewhetherpeoplehadtrustincivilservants, theywerepresentedwiththefollowingstatement:“Most civilservantscanbetrustedtodowhatisbestforthecoun- Our individual-level data come from surveys collected try.”Respondentscouldanswer“stronglyagree,”“agree,” aspartoftheInternationalSocialSurveyProgram(ISSP) “neitheragreenordisagree,”“disagree,”and“stronglydis- in 1996 as part of a study called Role of Government agree.”Thisquestiongaugespeople’strustinafairlyspe- III. Countries that provided the most important sur- cific set of actors—more specific, certainly, than asking vey items and that had a sufficient number of cases aboutthepoliticalsystemasawhole.Generally,thisques- for multivariate analysis included Australia, Canada, tionandsimilaronesaimedatparliament,thecourts,the the Czech Republic, Germany, Great Britain, Hungary, armed services, etc., have been considered indicators of Ireland, Italy, Japan, Latvia, New Zealand, Norway, support for regime institutions (cf. Klingemann 1999). Russia,Slovenia,Sweden,andtheUnitedStates.Thus,we The variable was coded as a five-category measure that wereabletoemploysurveysfromadiversesetof coun- rankedtrustfrom1to5,with5denotingthemosttrust- trieswithwidelyvaryingpoliticalcultures,structures,and ingresponse. histories. Figure1showsthedistributionofpositiveresponses tothetwosupportvariables(percentagessayingthatthe DependentVariables politicalsystemworkswellandthatcivilservantscanbe trusted)acrossthecountriesincludedinthisstudy.First, Ourdependentvariableindicatorstapintodifferentdi- on average, 60 percent of citizens have a positive opin- mensions of support for the political regime, with one ionabouttheirpoliticalsystem,butonly23percenthave geared toward general evaluations of the system’s per- trustincivilservants.Second,thegraphshowsthatthere formance and the other asking respondents to evaluate is considerable cross-national variation in people’s atti- particular institutional actors—civil servants.4 To mea- tudes toward government. Specifically, positive perfor- sure performance evaluations, respondents were asked: manceevaluationsrangefromaround18percentsaying “Allinall,howwellorbadlydoyouthinkthesystemof thatthesystemworkswellinRussiaallthewaytomore democracy in (country) works these days?” The answer than88percentsayingthesystemworkswellinNorway, categories were: “It works well and needs no changes; it withmostcountries’levelsof positiveevaluationsrang- works well, but needs some changes; it does not work ingsomewherebetween60and70percent.Thegraphalso wellandneedsalotofchanges;itdoesnotworkwelland showsthatthereisconsiderablevariationacrosscontem- needstobecompletelychanged.”Theseanswercategories porary democracies with regard to whether people feel rangedfrom1to4,with4denotingthemostpositiveand theycantrustcivilservantstodowhatisbestforthecoun- 1themostnegativeevaluation. try.WhiletheIrishandtheNorwegiansarequitetrusting This measure does not capture citizen attitudes to- oftheircivilservantsat52and37.5percent,respectively, ward democracy as an ideal; instead, it focuses on peo- Italians, Swedes, Hungarians, Czechs, and Russians are ple’s responses to the actual process of democratic gov- considerablylesstrusting,withonlyabout10–15percent ernance and their attitudes toward a country’s “consti- ofthemexpressingtrustincivilservants. tutionalreality”(Fuchs,Guidorossi,andSvensson1995, 328). Using Easton’s categories, this indicator has been IndependentVariables identifiedasameasureofsupportfortheperformanceof thepoliticalregime(cf.Klingemann1999;Norris1999). Corruption. Recall that our primary goal is to test the Becauseitallowsustoconnectcorruption,whichwear- following relationships: (1) between corruption and at- gue to be an indicator of system performance, with an titudes toward government, (2) between political alle- evaluation of the performance of the system in the eyes giances and attitudes toward government, and (3) the of itscitizens,thismeasureisparticularlyusefulforour interactive effects of corruption and political allegiance purposes. onattitudestowardgovernment.Toestimatetheimpact of corruption, we need to connect attitudes about gov- ernmentwithinformationaboutcorruptpracticesinthe 4Using a single item indicator, as opposed to a multi-item con- respondent’scountry.Todoso,wecombinedacountry- struct,maycompromisethereliabilityofourdependentvariables. Note,however,thataddingmoreitemstoanindicatorhasnodirect level measure of corruption with the individual-level effectonitsvalidity(Guilford1954;KerlingerandLee2000).More- surveys. over,anunreliabledependentvariabledoesnotbiasregressionesti- OurmeasureofcorruptionisbasedontheCorrup- mates,butitmakesithardertoachievestatisticalsignificance(King, Keohane,andVerba1994). tionPerceptionIndex(CPI)developedbyTransparency 96 CHRISTOPHERJ.ANDERSONANDYULIYAV.TVERDOVA FIGURE1 EvaluationsofPoliticalSystemPerformanceandTrustinCivil Servants n 100 o ni 90 pi O 80 e v siti 70 o P 60 a g 50 n ssi 40 e xpr 30 E e 20 g a nt 10 e c er 0 P Australia Britain CaCnzaedcahRepublicGermany Hungary Ireland Italy Japan LatNvieawZealand Norway Russia Slovenia SwedUennitedStates Evaluation of System Trust in Civil Servants International, the most frequently used and most com- fortotallycleanfromcorruptionand10indicatesahighly prehensivemeasureofcorruptionaroundtheworld.The corruptcountry.7 CPIisacompositemeasurebasedontheassessmentsof Figure 2 shows the distribution of the corruption country experts as well as the views of many different index across the countries included in this study. As in individuals in each country who encounter corruption thecaseofthepoliticalsupportquestions,thedatashow indifferingwaysandinavarietyof contexts.Itisbased considerable cross-national variation in corruption. On onthescoresofratingspublishedbyinternationalorga- average,thecountriesincludedintheanalysiswereonly nizations and constitutes the average of the scores from moderatelycorruptat3.1.Thehighestlevelsofcorruption thosesurveys(seetheappendixfordetails).5Weusedthe werefoundinRussiaandLatvia(atover7onthe10-point corruption index measured for 1996 to match with our scale), whereas the cleanest countries turned out to be survey data.6 The original measure runs from 0 (highly corrupt)to10(absolutelycleanfromcorruption).Tofa- 7Ourresearchdesignrequiresthatourmeasureofcorruptionbe cilitatetheinterpretationofthestatisticalresultsinlight exogenoustoattitudestowardthesystem.Giventhatourcorrup- tion index is a measure of public and elite perceptions of cor- ofourhypotheses,wereversedthescalesuchthat0stands ruption(albeitaggregatedatthesystemlevel),itispossiblethat ourmeasureof corruption,atleastinsmallpart,isendogenous to the model. To test whether this was the case, we conducted a Hausman two-step test of exogeneity (Hausman 1978; see also PindyckandRubinfeld,1991,303–05).Inthefirststage,corrup- tionwasregressedonallindependent(exogenous)variablesofthe 5Althoughresearchershaveevaluatedthevalidity,reliability,and originalmodelplussomeinstrumentalvariablesthataredirectly robustnessoftheindexwithextremelypositiveresults(Lancaster relatedtothesuspectedendogenousvariablebutnotthedependent andMontinola1997),itisnotwithoutlimitationsbecauseitas- variableofinterest.Followingpreviousresearch(cf.Sandholtzand sumes that corruption is a one-dimensional phenomenon that Koetzle2000;Treisman2000),weusedtrade(%GDP)andamea- can be measured along a single continuum, and that those sur- sureofeconomicfreedomasexogenouspredictorsofcorruption veyed operate with a similar definition of corruption. However, inthefirststageregression(yieldinganR2of.87).Theestimated given that there usually are few witnesses to corruption and be- residualsfromthisregressionwerethendefinedtoforma“residual cause those who have knowledge of it typically have an interest variable”thatwasaddedtothemodelexplainingsystemsupport inkeepingitsecret,people’sperceptionsarelikelytobemoreac- inasecondstageregression.Iftheresidualvariableisstatistically curatethananygovernment-sanctionedcountofcorruptactions significantinthesecondstageregression,evenwhencontrolling (Treisman2000). forthepresumedexogenousvariable,thentheregressoris,infact, 6Because the CPI for 1996 did not include data for Latvia and endogenous. Results from the second stage regression show that Slovenia,weusedthedatafor1998instead,assumingthatcorrup- theresidualvariablefailedtoachievestatisticalsignificance(sys- tionlevelsareunlikelytochangeradicallyovertime.Inspectionof temperformancemodel:(cid:2)=.011(s.e.:.011);trustincivilservants trendsintheCPIdataforothercountriesconfirmsthatthiswasa model:(cid:2)=.030(s.e.:.019).Wethereforeconcludedthatthecor- reasonableassumption(seealsoTreisman2000). ruptionvariablewasnotendogenoustosystemsupport. CORRUPTIONANDATTITUDESTOWARDGOVERNMENT 97 FIGURE2 Cross-NationalLevelsofCorruption 10 h) g 9 hi = 0 8 1 w; 7 o =l 0 6 n ( o 5 pti ru 4 r o C 3 of el 2 v e L 1 0 Australia Britain CaCzneacdhaRepublicGermany Hungary Ireland Italy Japan LatNvieaw Zealand Norway RussiaSlovenia SwedUennitedStates NewZealandandSwedenatlessthan1.Oftheremaining voted for the current government, he was coded as be- countries,sixscoredbetween1and2(Australia,Britain, longingtothemajority(coded1);allotherswerecoded Canada,Germany,Ireland,andNorway),twoscoredbe- 0.Toexaminetheinteractionbetweenlevelsofcorruption tween 2 and 3 (Japan and the United States), and four andpoliticalallegianceonattitudestowardthesystem,we (theCzechRepublic,Hungary,Italy,andSlovenia)scored alsoaddedaninteractionterm(majority-minoritystatus ∗ higherthanfourbutlowerthanRussiaandLatviaatthe corruption)totheequation. highend. ControlVariables Politicalallegiance:Majorityandminoritystatus. The political majority and minority status variable was cre- We also sought to control for a variety of factors that ated with the help of a question asking which party or have been found to predict support for the political presidentialcandidatetheindividualhadvotedforinthe system in previous analyses. Including these variables last national election. Relying on Keesing’s Archives, the avoidsdrawingfaultyinferencesduetospuriousnessthat Political Handbook of the World, and the European Jour- can result from omitting relevant variables. The control nal of Political Research, the next step was to determine variables fall into two categories: system-level variables whowontheelectionineachcountry.Wethencombined and individual-level variables. At the system level, these theinformationabouttheperson’spastvotewiththein- formation about the party in power.8 If the respondent comesandrecalledelectionoutcomes.Inoursample,49.1percent of respondents reported voting for the majority parties in their countries;thisisslightlyhigherthanthe46.6percentofactualvot- 8Themeasureofmajority-minoritystatusmaysufferfromthepo- ersforwinningparties.Asthesefiguressuggest,overreportingis tentialproblemofbiasedrecalloroverreportsfavoringthevictori- thusslightlymorepervasivethanunderreporting.Thismeansthat ousparty(cf.Wright1993).Notethat,dependingontheelectoral ourresultsarelikelytounderstatethetrueeffectsofpoliticalma- cycle,over-andunderreportingbotharepossible,giventhatgov- jority and minority status. Another source of bias could exist if ernments’honeymoonisusuallyfollowedbyadeclineinpopular voterswhoaremore(less)trustingofthepoliticalsystemwould supportduringtheelectoralterm.Whenpresent,overreportingis over(under)reporttheirvotefortheincumbentgovernment.Ifsuch likely to be less problematic because it implies that some of the abiasexists,thereshouldbearelationshipbetweenlevelsoftrust respondentswhoclaimtohavevotedforthevictorinfactdidnot. andover(under)reportofthevote.Wethereforecalculatedthedif- Inthiscase,someoftherespondentsclassifiedasbelongingtothe ferencebetweenthereportedandtheactualvoteinacountryand majority are, in fact, members of the minority. This would lead correlatedthisfigure(ameasureofover/underreportofthevote) tounderestimatingthetrueeffectofhavingvotedforthewinning withthelevelsofsystemsupport.Thesecorrelationswerestatisti- partyorparties.Asacheckontheaccuracyofthereportedvote, callyinsignificant(trustincivilservants:.05(p=.9);democracy we compared the aggregate distributions of actual election out- satisfaction:−.2(p=.9)). 98 CHRISTOPHERJ.ANDERSONANDYULIYAV.TVERDOVA include current macroeconomic performance,9 eco- ahigherprobabilityofTypeIerrors(seealsoZorn2001). nomic development,10 democratic longevity, and level Toestimateourmodels,wethereforereliedonstatistical of democracy.11 At the level of individual respondents, techniquesdevelopedspecificallyformodelingmultilevel we controlled for such factors as political interest, elec- datastructures(SteenbergenandJones2002).13 toralparticipation,12socioeconomicstatus,employment Several specific issues may pose problems for infer- situation, and a standard set of demographic variables. ence because of the multilevel nature of our data. First, Codingproceduresanddescriptivestatisticsforallvari- theinterceptsmaybevariableacrosscountries;failureto ablesarelistedintheappendix. controlforthismayresultinbiasedestimates.Specifically, ifinterceptsarevariable,wemaybeoverestimatingtheef- fect of corruption on system support, as the corruption AnalysisandResults coefficientcouldbecapturingboththetrueeffectsofcor- ruptionaswellasothercountry-specificeffects.14 Asec- Our research design requires that we combine infor- ondaryconcernisthattheindividuallevelvariablesmay mation at the level of respondents (micro-level) and haveunequalslopesacrossnations.Inthiscaseapooled countries (macro-level). This means that our data have estimator may be biased for each particular country. A a multilevel structure where one unit of analysis (vot- thirdconcernrelatestotherobustnessof ourinferences ers) is nested within the other (countries) (Bryk and basedonpotentiallyinefficientstandarderrorsresulting Raudenbush1992).Thistypeofdatastructurecangener- from potential clustering (cf. Zorn 2001). To deal with ateanumberofstatisticalproblems,suchasnonconstant theseissues,multilevelmodelingtechniquesallowforesti- variance and clustering. Failure to recognize the hierar- matingvaryinginterceptsandslopes,produceasymptoti- chicalnatureofthedatacanleadtounderestimatingstan- callyefficientstandarderrors,andprovideforadirectesti- darderrors—particularlyatthemacro-level—and,thus, mationofvariancecomponentsateachlevelofthemodel. Below,weshowthecoefficientsofinterest(constants andindependentvariables),aswellasthevariancecom- 9TheISSPsurveysusedheredidnotfurnishvariablesmeasuring ponentsateachlevelofourdata(individualandcountry- people’s perceptions of economic performance. Thus, to the ex- tentthateconomicperceptionsdeviatefromobjectivereality,our level).Theseestimationsallowustoestablish(a)whether estimatesof the“true”economiceffectsmaybebiased.Togeta corruption is a significant determinant of system sup- roughsenseofhowwellobjectiveeconomicconditionsmatchup portonceweallowtheinterceptstovaryacrosscountries witheconomicevaluations,weconstructedadatasetthatincluded theWestEuropeancountriesincludedinoursample(comparable and obtain better estimates of standard errors; and (b) datawerenotavailablefortheothercountries).Usingsurveydata whether our macro-variables explain a substantial pro- drawnfromtheEurobarometerandobjectiveeconomicindicators portionofthecountry-levelvarianceinorderforustobe for1991–95,thecorrelationofgrowthandretrospectiveevaluations abletoclaimthatwehaveminimizedapotentialomitted ofnationaleconomywasarobustandstatisticallysignificant.68 (p<.01);thecorrelationofgrowthandretrospectiveevaluations variablebias. ofpersonalfinancialsituationwasasimilarlystrong.67(p<.01). 10A number of scholars have argued that levels of economic de- AnalysisofVariance velopmentareassociatedwithlevelsofdemocracy.However,most analystshavenotdrawnadirectlinkbetweeneconomicdevelop- mentanddemocraticlegitimacy;insteadtheyhavefocusedona Todetermine,first,whetherthereissignificantvariation moreindirectconnectionofthetwoviademocraticstability.We insystemsupportattheindividualandcountrylevels,we alsoincludedameasureofdevelopmentbecausecorruptionismore estimated an ANOVA model that decomposes the vari- prevalentinlessdevelopedcountries,anditisconceivablethatany anceinthedependentvariables,where effectforcorruptiononsystemsupportmightpickupboththeac- tualeffectofcorruptionaswellastheeffectofacountry’swealth. Support =(cid:3) +(cid:4) +ε ij 00 0j ij 11Democraticlongevitywasmeasuredbythenumberofyearsof In this model, (cid:3) is the grand mean of support. The continuousdemocracysince1920.Weusedtheloggedvaluesofthis 00 variabletoreducetheeffectsofextremecases,thoughtheresultsdo sourcesofcross-nationalvariation,whichcauseparticu- notchangenoticeablywhenthesimplescaleisusedintheanalyses. larcountriestodeviatefromthismean,arecontainedin 12Followingtheargumentthatparticipationenhancesfeelingsof trust and external efficacy (and vice versa) (cf. Finkel 1987), we 13WeusedMLwiN1.10.0006(2000)toestimatethesemodels(see includedavariabledistinguishingvotersandnonvoterstocontrol alsoRasbashetal.1999). fordifferencesattributabletohavingparticipatedintheelection. Includingthisvariablealsoensuresthatweproperlyattributeany 14Awaytocapturevariableinterceptswouldbetointroduceaseries effectsforbeingamongthemajorityandminority.Weexpectthat ofdummyvariablesforeachcountryinthedataset,butone.In thosewhodidnotparticipateintheelection(coded1)willhave ourcase,however,thissolutionisimpossible,becauseof perfect morenegativeattitudestowardthepoliticalsystemthanthosewho multicollinearity among the macro-level variables and the set of did(coded0). countrydummies. CORRUPTIONANDATTITUDESTOWARDGOVERNMENT 99 TABLE1 ANOVA minoritystatus,aswellastheinteractiveeffectsof these variables,onevaluationsofpoliticalsystemperformance. Estimate We show four models: Models 1 and 2 are random in- tercept multilevel maximum likelihood IGLS (Iterative EvaluationofSystem Trustin GeneralizedLeastSquares)modelswithandwithoutthe Parameter Performance CivilServants interaction terms of corruption and majority-minority FixedEffects status. In addition, we report the results of parametric ∗∗∗ ∗∗∗ Constant 2.555 2.561 andnonparametricbootstrappingestimations(Models3 (.082) (.079) and4).16 Theresultsfromthebootstrappedestimations Variance servetoexaminetherobustnessoftheIGLSestimates,in ∗∗∗ ∗∗ Components .107 .098 particulartheresultsinvolvingthemacro-levelvariables Country-level (.038) (.035) (suchascorruption),giventhatourlevel2variablesare ∗∗∗ ∗∗∗ .417 1.069 measured at the level of countries and thus for a fairly Individual-level (.004) (.010) smallnumberofcases. −2loglikelihood 41,886.88 62,023.61 Regardlessofestimationmethod,theresultsprovide unambiguousevidenceinsupportofourmainhypothe- Notes:Entriesaremaximumlikelihood(IGLS)estimates;standard sis:individualsincountrieswithhigherlevelsofcorrup- errorsinparentheses. ∗p<0.05;∗∗p<0.01;∗∗∗p<.001. tionevaluatetheperformanceofthepoliticalsystemmore negatively.Inshort,corruptionbreedsdiscontentwiththe (cid:4) ,andε containssourcesofinter-individualvariation. performanceofthepoliticalsystem.Theresultsobtained 0j ij withtherandominterceptmodels(Models1and2)show Theargumentthatbothlevelsof analysisareimportant thatthecorruptioncoefficientwasstatisticallyandsub- for understanding attitudes toward the political system stantively significant. As importantly, the bootstrapped issupportedifbothvariancecomponentsarestatistically results shown in Models 3 and 4, which reexamine the significant(cf.SteenbergenandJones2002). resultsofthefullyinteractiveModel2,indicatethatthese Table 1 shows the ML estimates of the grand mean resultswereextremelyrobust.Visualinspectionofthedis- andthevariancecomponents.Bothvariancecomponents tributionofthecoefficientsforcorruptionshowedthem arestatisticallysignificant,suggestingthatthereissignif- tobenormallydistributed. icantvarianceinsystemsupportatbothlevelsof analy- Ourresultsalsoshowthatthosewhovotedforparties sis.ResultsoftheANOVAmodelshowthatcountry-level in power had more positive attitudes toward the politi- varianceisproportionallymuchsmallerthanindividual- calsystem.Asexpected,citizensinthepoliticalmajority level variance. Given that the data are measured at the thoughtthepoliticalsystemworkedbetterthandidthose individual level, this is not surprising (Steenbergen and intheminority.Again,allfourmodelsusingdifferentesti- Jones 2002, 231). Specifically, individual-level variance mationtechniquesshowedthatthisresultwassignificant constitutes79.6percentofthetotalvarianceinthegeneral andextremelyrobust. systemsupportmodel,whereasthispartofthevarianceis To better understand the estimated substantive im- evengreaterinthetrustofcivilservantsmodel(91.6per- pactof thevariablesof interest,wecalculatedhowaver- cent).15TheresultsoftheANOVAmodelindicateclearly agerespondents’evaluationsofthepoliticalsystemvaried that there is significant variation in system support for both dependent variables with regard to both levels of analysis. Thus, we now turn to the question of whether 16Bootstrappingproceduresestimatethesamplingdistributionof themodelwehavespecifiedcanaccountforthisvariance. astatistic,treatingthesampleasapopulation.Wepresentresults estimatedbybothparametricandnonparametricbootstrapping. Intheparametricbootstrapprocedure,MLwiNresamplesfromthe TheEffectsofCorruptionandPolitical residualdistributionspecifiedbythemodelateachlevel,whichin ourparticularmodelisassumedtobenormal.Themodelisthenre- AllegianceonEvaluations fittedusingtheseresponses.Intheparametricbootstrapprocedure ofSystemPerformance confidenceintervalsareestimatedinaccordancewiththenormal distributionsamplingtheoryandareeasytointerpret.However, Directeffects. Table2showstheresultsofseveralmodels sincewedidnothaveanystrongreasonstobelievethatourresiduals estimatingthedirecteffectsofcorruptionandmajority- aredistributednormally,wealsoperformednonparametricboot- strapping,whichdoesnotrequireanydistributionalassumptions. However,notethatthemorecomplexcomputationofconfidence 15Thesecalculationsarebasedontheratiosofeachvariancecom- intervalsinthenonparametricbootstrapprocedurecallsformore ponentrelativetothetotalvarianceinsystemsupport(cf.Brykand cautionwhendrawinginferencesonthebasisofresultsobtained Raudenbush1992;SnijdersandBosker1999). withthisprocedure(Mooney1996). 100 CHRISTOPHERJ.ANDERSONANDYULIYAV.TVERDOVA TABLE2 EffectsofCorruptionandPoliticalAllegianceonEvaluationsofPoliticalSystem Performancein16Democracies EvaluationsofSystemPerformance Model1 Model2 Model3 Model4 Random RandomIntercept Parametric Non-Parametric IndependentVariable InterceptModel ModelwithInteraction Bootstrapping Bootstrapping FixedEffects Constant 2.423∗∗∗ 2.542∗∗∗ 2.619∗∗∗ 2.586∗∗∗ (.475) (.451) (.522) (.572) Corruption −.078∗∗∗ −.092∗∗∗ −.101∗∗∗ −.092∗∗∗ (.023) (.022) (.023) (.024) Majority-minoritystatus .149∗∗∗ .042∗∗ .042∗∗ .043∗∗ (1=majority,0=minority) (.011) (.018) (.017) (.018) Corruption∗majority-minority .037∗∗∗ .037∗∗∗ .037∗∗∗ status (.005) (.005) (.005) Nonvoter −.021 −.022∗ −.022∗ −.022∗ (1=didnotvote,0=voted) (.013) (.012) (.012) (.012) Socioeconomicstatus .059∗∗∗ .058∗∗∗ .058∗∗∗ .057∗∗∗ (high=highsocialclass) (.005) (.005) (.005) (.005) Interestinpolitics .020∗∗∗ .021∗∗∗ .020∗∗∗ .021∗∗∗ (high=greatinterest) (.005) (.005) (.005) (.005) Employmentstatus .080∗∗ .079∗∗ .080∗∗∗ .079∗∗ (high=employed) (.026) (.026) (.023) (.026) Gender −.065∗∗∗ −.065∗∗∗ −.065∗∗∗ −.065∗∗∗ (1=female) (.011) (.011) (.011) (.010) Age .000 .000 .000 .000 (high=old) (.000) (.000) (.000) (.000) Education .012∗∗ .011∗∗ .011∗ .011∗∗ (high=higheducation) (.004) (.004) (.005) (.004) GNPpercapita .013∗∗ .013∗∗ .011∗ .013∗ (.005) (.005) (.006) (.006) Economicgrowth .032∗ .033∗ .030 .032∗ (percentchangeinGDP) (.017) (.016) (.019) (.019) Democraticage −.032 −.011 .004 −.010 (inyearssince1920,logged) (.054) (.052) (.057) (.052) Democracyscore −.037 −.057 −.062 −.065 (FreedomHouseindex) (.078) (.074) (.072) (.117) VarianceComponents Country-level .017∗∗ .016∗∗ .023∗∗ .024∗ (.006) (.006) (.010) (.012) Individual-level .384∗∗∗ .383∗∗∗ .382∗∗∗ .382∗∗∗ (.005) (.005) (.005) (.004) N 13,867 13,867 13,867 13,867 −2loglikelihood 26142.230 26086.050 27537.440 27597.070 Notes: Estimates are maximum likelihood estimates (IGLS) and bias corrected bootstrap estimates; standard errors in parentheses. Bootstrap estimates and standard errors are computed using resampling of the residuals with 5 sets of 300 replicates in parametric bootstrappingand5setsof500replicatesinnon-parametricbootstrapping. ∗p<.05;∗∗p<.01;∗∗∗p<.001.
Description: