Journal of Graphic Novels and Comics ISSN: 2150-4857 (Print) 2150-4865 (Online) Journal homepage: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/rcom20 Combining the rhythms of comics and picturebooks: thoughts and experiments Rebecca Palmer To cite this article: Rebecca Palmer (2014) Combining the rhythms of comics and picturebooks: thoughts and experiments, Journal of Graphic Novels and Comics, 5:3, 297-310, DOI: 10.1080/21504857.2014.926955 To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/21504857.2014.926955 Published online: 06 Jun 2014. Submit your article to this journal Article views: 52 View related articles View Crossmark data Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at http://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=rcom20 Download by: [Radcliffe Infirmary] Date: 14 October 2015, At: 03:08 JournalofGraphicNovelsandComics,2014 Vol.5,No.3,297–310,http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/21504857.2014.926955 Combining the rhythms of comics and picturebooks: thoughts and experiments Rebecca Palmer* FacultyofArts,LawandSocialScience,AngliaRuskinUniversity,Cambridge,UK (Received 7January2014;accepted15May2014) The quality of rhythm is intrinsic to the study of both comics and picturebooks. 5 However, the focus of theorists and practitioners carries a different emphasis for 1 0 each narrative form, visual rhythm being dominant where the comic is concerned, 2 r while analysis of picturebooks focuses on the dynamic rhythm of text and image e b working in concert. I suggest that this divergence is in part a reflection of a key o ct difference between the two forms of visual narrative, for a comic is made with the O private, silent reader in mind, whereas a picturebook anticipates a collective, vocal 4 1 reading. Yet picturebooks and comics also share substantial common ground, the 8 combinationofconventionsfrombothformssuggestingfurtherpossibilitiesforstory- 0 3: telling. This article offers a study through practice of the potential to create effective 0 at newrhythmsforcomic/picturebook crossovers inmyownwork. y] Keywords:rhythm;pace;musicalsequence;visualnarrative;crossover text r a m r fi n I Though they have developed as distinct forms of visual narrative, comics and picture- e ff books clearly share substantial common ground, and often borrow and quote from one cli another.Infactitcansometimesbedifficulttodefinewithcertaintywhichatextis.Inthe d a R last decades, the academic community has proposed various definitions for both comics y [ and picturebooks. Building on Will Eisner’s (2008) description of comics as ‘sequential b d art’, Scott McCloud proposed his now-famous definition of comics as ‘juxtaposed e d pictorial and other images in a deliberate sequence, intended to convey information and/ a nlo or produce an aesthetic response in the viewer’ (1993, 5). Criticism has been levelled at w McCloud’s definition that questions not only its accuracy in formal terms but also its o D a-historical nature (Aaron Meskin gives a succinct account of these critiques in his essay ‘Defining Comics?’ [2007]). Other attempts to isolate the essential qualities of comics havebeenadvanced:someemphasisetheblendofwordandimage(Harvey1979);others, likeMcCloud,focusontheirvisuallysequentialnature(Groensteen[1999]2007;Hayman and Pratt 2005); while others again point to historical, commercial and cultural context (Sabin 2001) as well as commonalities of style (Pratt 2011). Differencesinterminologyaside,academicsworkinginthefieldofpicturebookshave generally been in agreement on the basic definition of the form, the interdependence of word and image being the common denominator and point of difference from illustrated booksandotherformsofchildren’sliterature(thisdistinctionisclearlyoutlinedbyMaria Nikolajeva and Carole Scott [2006]). Yet this definition also raises difficulties: what are we to make of a book that presents a visual narrative without words? Moreover, since *Email:[email protected] ©2014RebeccaPalmer 298 R. Palmer comics studies and picturebook studies have come more closely into contact, both have had to acknowledge that the collaboration of word and image as a distinct narrative medium is not unique to either form. Recent years have seen a growth in exchanges between the two areas of research. In 2012, the Children’s Literature Association Quarterly published the results of a symposium entitled ‘Why Comics Are and Are Not PictureBooks’,withcontributionsfromexpertsineachfield.Allgrapplewiththeformal distinctions between them, some proposing a revised taxonomy (Nodelman 2012; op de Beeck 2012), others preferring to frame their relationship in terms of genealogy (Nel 2012, 445). In speaking of the problematic nature of an attempt to define the essential characteristics of each form individually, especially in the light of their shared character- istics, Charles Hatfield and Craig Svonkin refer to Wittgenstein’s notion of family resem- blancestheoryanditsrecentimpactongenretheory.Wittgensteinsuggeststhat,ratherthan possessing an essential property in common, related things may be linked by a number of overlapping similarities (Hatfield and Svonkin 2012, 434). In doing so, they join Aaron 5 1 Meskin(2007)incallingintoquestionanyprojectwishingtodrawfixedlinestodemarcate 0 2 r either form. History shows that such lines are liable to provoke artists in any medium to e b muddle them and cross them, and book artists are no exception (the work of Shaun Tan o ct being only one much-cited example). But it follows that such perceived parameters are a O 4 prerequisite for transgression: in fact, to work within or against constraints is one of the 8 1 mostfruitfulcircumstancesforcreativethinkingandmaking.InMeskin’sview,theinterest 0 3: in identifying what is characteristic and conventional to each form lies in using that at 0 knowledge tounderstandhow artists use orrefusethem(2007, 376). As a maker ofvisual ] narrative,theinterestformeissimilarlynotinanydefinitionperse,butinthemechanicsof y ar each form as a structure to adapt and to question in the process. m r In the context of this essay and the practical experiments on which it reflects, my nfi attentionwasparticularlyfocussedonthedifferenceandtheoverlapintherelationshipof I e word, image and reading in comics and picturebooks. Having worked principally on f f cli narratives in comic form, where the words related to the images as dialogue, physically d a present on the page and in the images in a very different way to the words in a picture- R [ book,Iwascurioustoseehowthatinterweavingoftextandimage,bothinamaterialand y b narrative sense, would work in the context of a picturebook. I follow Jane Doonan’s d e (1993, 9) example in invoking the reader as a key part of the triad from which the d oa narrativesprings,sincethereaderorreadersandthewaytheyreadseemstometobeone wnl ofthemaindistinctionsbetweenthetwoforms,namelythereadingexperiencestheyeach Do imply. The point that all scholarship comparing the two forms returns to is the intended readership of the picturebook, which is understood by all to be inherently a form ‘for’ children,whetherthecontentissuitableornot.1Althoughpicturebooksmaybereadalone in solitude and silence, and in fact such readings have their own characteristics and benefits, as a form it is designed to be shared too, read out loud to or with others. Editors of picturebooks have described to me how they always test the words to see how theysoundoutloud.EricCarleascribesBillMartinJr’ssuccessasapicturebookwriterto thewayhewrotehisstories‘rhythmfirst’,testingoutthemeterbeforecomposingthetext (Marcus 2012, 71). Acomic,ontheotherhand,isbestsuitedtobereadinsilence.Comicsseemtoresist being read out: to do so flattens the dialogue and robs the sequence of its careful timing. Pace and rhythm is of central importance to both comics and picturebooks, but it is no surprise, given the different readers and readings they project, that the emphasis for theorists and practitioners should be on visual rhythm in comics, while the rhythm of the text as it is spoken plays a vital role in the picturebook, its ‘sayableness’ and Journal of Graphic Novels and Comics 299 momentum central tothe dynamicrelationship ofword and text (Shannon 1991). Clearly picturebookscanbeandarereadsilentlyandalone,andcomicsmightequallybereadout loud.Yetourvariedinteractionswiththemasreadersdonot,Ithink,precludetheirformal andmaterialadaptationforparticularkindsofreading.Theseriesofexperimentsinvisual storytelling I present here (Figures 1–8) isthe result of an ongoing curiosity to see if and howIcouldbridgethatdifference,creatingacomicdesignedtoreadoutloudthatisalso a picturebook in direct speech. Following the precedent set by Matt Madden’s 99 Ways to Tell a Story (2005) and Raymond Queneau’s Exercises in Style (1981; first published in 1947 as Exercices de Style), which inspired my own series, I set out to choose an incident as unremarkable as they had done. An event from my own childhood presented itself: bath time. There were six of us, and as many as possible would often be squeezed into the tub at once. Dad would come in to dry our hair. He would sing ‘The Grand Old Duke of York’, towelling your head from side to side, side to side, to the end of the rhyme. The rest of us played 5 1 until our turn came. The anecdote presented rich possibilities for investigating the inter- 0 2 r action of visual and verbal rhythms in narratives where image and word are mutually e b dependent for meaning. I also discovered that the complexity of representing so much o ct simultaneous activity in the same graphic space stretched my competence in a way that O 4 led to a firmer understanding of the structuring of visual narrative and its unique 8 1 capacities. Lastly, the story describes what Margaret Meek (1987, 21) calls a ‘literacy 3:0 event’ in my own childhood, and as such is useful in that it illustrates very palpably the at 0 linksbetweentheactive,socialexperiencesthatmakeuptheearlieststageofliteracy,and ] the journey into silent reading at a later stage that seems so much formed by that y ar beginning. What follows outlines my thoughts in relation to all three. I refer to my m r drawings throughout, but I intend that they should also speak for themselves. One by nfi one and in combination, they show one side of this investigation. In writing, I want to I e examine the other side: my thoughts as they developed in a reflexive relationship with f f cli these experiments in visual narrative, so that the reader can trace their mutual impact. d a Throughout the series, the rhythm of the nursery rhyme is a constant among many R [ variables, repeated in each experiment just as it was repeated for every child that got out y b ofthebath;repeated,infact,asasimplemelodyrecursinaseriesofmusicalvariations,a d e comparison to which I return. Nursery rhymes have featured in picturebooks since their d oa earliest beginnings, developed to their full comic potential with the illustrations of wnl Randolph Caldecott and others since. Introduced into a new narrative context, away Do from Mother Goose, the rhyme becomes one voice among many here, a strong basic rhythm under the less ordered cadences of the conversation and noise from the bath. My first drawings (Figure 1) were very literal, representing and reinforcing the rhythm of the rhyme with an echoing visual rhythm. I made broad brushstrokes and minimal, quick drawings of the figures, intending to embody something of the movement and energy of theactionandunderlinethebeatofthemarchthatisthesubjectoftherhyme.Thequality of mark-making in a still image can suggest movement very effectively, as the work of artistslikeQuentinBlakedemonstratessoclearly.Thedrawnlineisanindexofthehand that made it, which can invest what it represents with its own motion (Atkinson 2009, 271). Art Spiegelman ([1989] 2011) likes to describe comics as diagrams for conveying informationgraphicallyinthemosteffective(andaffective)way.ForSpiegelman([1989] 2011; 2004, v), clarity is key to communicating the story, taking precedence over aesthetics. So that the sequence would read as moment-to-moment action, and match the pace of the rhyme when sung or spoken aloud, I reduced the figures to the bare 300 R. Palmer (a) (b) 5 1 0 2 r e b o ct O 4 1 8 0 3: 0 at Figure1. Firsttrials inexpressing the rhythm ofthe text inthe rhythm ofthe image. ©Rebecca ] Palmer 2013. y r a m minimumnecessarytoconveythecharactersandmotion.Drawingthematspeedbrought r nfi me closer to that economy, whilst also creating a quickness of line that added to the I e animation of the figures. f f cli What interested me, looking at these drawings on a page together, line after line, was ad their relationship to musical notation. Both Eisner (2008, 26) and McCloud (2000, 206– R [ 207)linkthepatternofpanelsonapageofcomicstothepassingoftimerepresentedbya by musical score. Thierry Groensteen ([1999] 2007, 45) draws a parallel between the ‘basic ed heartbeat’rhythm‘imposedbythesuccessionofframes’andtherhythmorbeatinmusic. d oa It is perhaps not surprising that, attempting to show movement that matched the irrepres- nl sible beat and drive of the rhyme, my first impulse was to make a kind of score. Musical w o notationisdesignedforinterpretationbythosewhocandecipherandinterpretitasmusic. D Even as it exists alongside others in the same time and place, as a pattern on the page, each note is a new moment in time, like the panels of a comic. The magic of dots and dashestransformedtobecomemusichassomethingincommonwiththewayacomiccan conjureupcharactersandtheirstories.Theparallelbetweenascoreandacomicservedas a reminder to me that it is the reading that creates the rhythms. A musical score can be beautiful in itself but, if we know what it is for, we know that its purpose is to be translated into sound. The composer’s creation is fully realised only when the musician interprets the score. The physicality of a comic or picturebook, where what I see on the pagehasanaestheticaswellasfunctionalquality,candistractfromanunderstandingthat the point of transition from page to mind is the place where the story ‘happens’. This makes sense of the idea that a static image on the page can have rhythm or pace. The rhythms are perceived, they play on the mind’s agility in making associations and its aptitude for metaphorical thinking (Lakoff and Johnson 2003, 6). If a musical score is unintelligible to us, the graphic pattern still suggests a rhythm. Journal of Graphic Novels and Comics 301 QueneauwasfirstpromptedtobeginhisExercisesinStylebyaperformanceofBach’s Art of the Fugue (Campbell-Sposito 1997, 6). In a fugue, the melody is picked up by a numberofinstrumentsinsuccession,eachonegivingitsownparticulartoneandcharacter tothetheme.Thevisualparallelbetweenmyfirstdrawingsandamusicalscoreledmeto associate the series I was making with a set of musical variations, a useful analogy for understanding what I do as story-teller.2 I am always pre-occupied by a wish to recount lived experiences so that they are recognisable and ‘ring true’. Naturally, this leads tothe temptationtouseawordlike‘capture’todescribehowIgoaboutdoingthat,whichframes myprocessasakindofuninflectedrecordingoftheworld.ButthestoriesItellareoften fictional,andevenhere,wherewhatIamrepresentingreallyhappened,theimpossibilityof simplycapturingitonthepageisevidenteveninmyfirstattempt.Marryingtherhythmof the nursery rhyme – and the regularity of action that accompanies it – to the haphazard noise and activity of children in the bath was a challenging task, more so than I had anticipated. So many elements of text and image must be brought together in a way that 5 1 reads simply and clearly, evoking the scene in the mind of the reader with all the 0 2 r immediacy of a pop-up tent. There is more than one form that it could take, variations e b on the theme that will each bring the scene to life in a different way. It falls to me as o ct storytellertoorchestrate,averbthatdescribesmuchmoreeffectivelymyeffortstoweave O 4 thestrandsoftheeventtogethersothattheyactonandwitheachother,communicatingas 1 8 a whole. It also acknowledges the construction of the narrative, whether it is fictional or 3:0 not. When I tell a story in comics, I want to be ‘a window on something’, as Joe Sacco at 0 (quoted in McGrath 2004) puts it. I have always thought of myself as effaced in the ] process, because it is the loss of self-consciousness in the intuitive playing-out of char- y ar actersandeventsthatallowsmetotellstoriesatall.ButSaccoscrupulouslyrevealsitasan m r activerolebyincludinghimselfasparticipant/observerinhiscomics.Itisimportantnotto nfi vanishcompletelyfrommyownfieldofvision:youhavetocreateawindowbeforeothers I e can look through it, and of course it always frames the view from a particular aspect. To f f cli identify myself with the window is to acknowledge that my role as storyteller is itself a d a construction. The difficulty of representing time and space in two dimensions brings you R [ up short againstthenecessity to invent structuresthat will invite thereader’s imagination y b in,andsoevokeeventsmostvividly.Theparticulardifficultyinrepresentingthisbathroom ed scene ‘in the flat’ also brought me face to face with my capabilities and intentions, with d oa myselfas director and creator, notconduit ormedium. wnl Thoughunconsciously,Imusthaveconceivedofmyfirstdrawingsasthebaselineof Do ascore,intendingtorepresentthechildreninthebathaboveorbesidethatregularactivity likeanotherstave,inthehopethatthetwowouldreadassimultaneousevents.Figures2a and 2b show the sequence that was supposed to act in this way alongside Figure 1, depictingthechildreninthebath,althoughthefacttheyareinabathisnotentirelyclear.I wrote the text of the speech bubble with a looser rhythm, attempting intermingled conversations that follow naturally from one another but nevertheless fit a more subtle meterwhenspoken.Thechangeinsizeofthepanelsandtheirmergingwasmeanttohave a similar effect, the physical structure of the frames shifting to accommodate the move- ments of the children. Unfortunatelythedramaofthehair-dryingandthedramainthebath donotreadasa whole: visually, there is nothing to connect them, so my attempt to highlight the contrast in rhythm is quite ineffectual. I was trying to show too much, failing to trust the reader’s imagination enough. My initial drawings of the hair-drying, in which each tug of the towelisrepresentedliterallyindirecttransposition,aretoocloseandrepeatedtoooftento givearealsenseofvigorousmovement.Justasinmusic,certainnotesmustbeaccented: 302 R. Palmer (a) (b) 5 1 0 2 r e b o ct O 4 1 8 0 3: 0 at ] y r a m r fi Figure2. Experimentscontrastinganarrativesequencewithandwithouttext.©RebeccaPalmer n I 2013. e f f cli d Withoutaccentthereisnolife.Thebeatbecomesmonotonousandwearisome.Musicwithout a R accent lacks coherence, and movement becomes aimless where there is no impulse. [ y Conversely, if every note, word or movement is stressed, the result has even less meaning. b d (Driver1936,34) e d a o Every maker of comics knows the importance of identifying which moments to show, nl w leaving the rest to happen in the gutter, as McCloud describes (1993, 64), implied rather o D thanstated.Themoreourmindsareengagedbysupplementingthenarrative,themorewe are invested in what is represented (Gombrich [1960] 1977, 184–185). (a) (b) Figure3. Thisfantasy sequencedevelopedfromthepatternofbrush strokesinFigure1b,which reminded meoftheswingingarmsof thesoldiersintherhyme.©Rebecca Palmer2013. Journal of Graphic Novels and Comics 303 As well as deciding what would be depicted and what could be left out, I had to reconsider the quantity of text that I wanted to include. Though they interact with the panels as a graphic counterpoint to their rhythm, the speech bubbles of Figure 2a intrude intotheactionandthewordsfixittoofirmlyinplace,distractingandconfusingthereader by placing emphasis on a storyline that should rather be understood as an incidental part of the whole. When the activity in the bath is allowed to exist without text, it is more effective, and I was struck in a new way by the wisdom of wordless picturebooks and comics like Jan Ormerod’s well-known Sunshine (1981) and Moonlight (1982). These bookswelcomethekindofsolitarycontemplationOrmerod’ssmallprotagonistisengaged in at the start of one and the end of the other. However, they are equally effective when readoutloud:infact,thepicturesinviteyoungreaderstointerpretwhatishappeningand to give the characters voice. Thus the book is ‘read out’ in the most conversational and direct way possible, the child reader inhabiting and owning the story and its characters without any need for speech bubbles. 5 1 So we return to the idea of reading a story out loud as a shared experience. Nothing 0 2 r could be nicer than to be read to, or to be told a story: Daniel Pennac (2006) gives a e b glowingdescriptionofthepleasureandvalueinthat.Buttoparticipateinthestorytelling o ct is also enjoyable, and valuable, both for adults and children. Much writing on children’s O 4 interaction with picturebooks emphasises their equal, if not superior, perceptiveness in 8 1 interpretingpictures(amongstothersMeek1987,10–11;NikolajevaandScott2006,261; 0 3: Arizpe and Styles 2002, 26). The picturebook, with simple subtlety, often addresses its ] at 0 iomblaigveiosutsoathdeulytowunhgorreeaaddesr,thwehmile(Gthiebswonor2d0s,1a0t,c1o0u2n)t.e3rpFoiginutr,ebse4loanngdb6yaasttseomcipattioansitmoitlhaer y ar interaction.Bothadvancedandearlyreadersaregivenpowertotellthestory,thepictures m r and words weighing equally in that telling. The words on the page may be supplemented nfi by many outside it; comments, corrections, extrapolations, supposings…. The book I e remains the same but each telling is different. Such communal reading reminds me that f f cli the text of a comic is closer to the script for a film or play than to that of a picturebook. d a Making a distinction between reading to and reading with, as Meek (1987, 26) does, is R [ key, for, though comics may be ‘well-nigh impossible’ to read to someone, they can be y b very satisfying to read together. Their visual nature invites a joint pouring-over and d e pointing-out of detail (Sabeti 2011, 143) and it is only the fact that the pacing takes d oa place principally through the rhythm of panels that hampers a dramatic reading. wnl Thinkingaboutthepotentialforacomic/picturebookmeanttobereadthiswayhelped o me to decide how to proceed with my own variations. Initially I had focussed solely on D what I wanted to show, my eyes fixed on the transition from the scene in my memory to the page. But as the series grew, I began to turn my attention on the point of commu- nication. The page had to convey the scene clearly and succinctly, without muddle. Pinpointing the essence or core of what I wanted to tell showed the way to certain forms that would allow me to do so. What was the characteristic of my childhood bath- timethatIwantedthereadertograsp,theonequalitythatwouldservetobringthewhole to life? The mingling of many voices, the interruptive clamour and hurly burly of the family together inthebathroomwas at theheart of it. Ido notknowwhether mywish to orchestrateitinthiscontextplays outearly fantasiesofcontrol asoneamongsomany in my family. One memory strikes me though: a car ride, me sitting in the back with the others, listening to various conversations crisscrossing. Someone asked a question, and the response from another conversation seemed to answer, the coincidence creating a hilarious nonsense (to me, at least). Figure 7 shows the sequence that comes closest to conveyingthissenseofthepotentialcomedyinwhichmanypeoplearebusilyabsorbedin 304 R. Palmer 5 1 0 2 r e b o ct O 4 1 8 0 3: 0 at ] y r a m r fi n I e f f cli d a R [ y b d e d a o nl w o D Figure4. Usingthevisualrhythmofpanelstoechoandemphasisetheregularbeatoftherhyme. ©RebeccaPalmer 2013. their own activities in the same space. That comedy is witnessed by the reader, as an outside observer, but they also participate in it, acting as characters. Read together, the shortexclamationsandsoundeffectsthatinterrupttheimperturbableGrandOldDukeare designedtobedecipheredeasilybyearlyreaderssothateveryonejoinsinwiththechaos. The short-hand drawing and splooshy wash denote the disorder and motion, their econ- omyclosertothemodeofdrawingincartoonsorcomics:intendedforclarity,maintaining momentum. The pronounced bow of the towel focuses attention on that action – perhaps Journal of Graphic Novels and Comics 305 5 1 0 2 r e b o ct O 4 1 8 0 3: 0 at ] y r a m r fi n I e f f cli Figure 5. A further trial exploring an alternative way to show the passage of time and the d a relationshipbetweentext andaction.©RebeccaPalmer 2013. R [ y b d de (a) (b) a o nl w o D Figure6. Figures6aand6baredesignedtobereadoneithersideofapageturn.6ashowsaslow buildupto 6b,adouble-pagespreadof mayhem thataccompaniesthe brisk rhythmof therhyme. ©RebeccaPalmer 2013.