ebook img

Cognition and Acquired Language Disorders PDF

366 Pages·2012·10.22 MB·English
by  
Save to my drive
Quick download
Download
Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.

Preview Cognition and Acquired Language Disorders

Richard K. Peach, PhD Professor Department of Communication Disorders and Sciences College of Health Sciences Rush University Chicago, Illinois Lewis P. Shapiro, PhD Professor School of Speech, Language, and Hearing Sciences College of Health and Human Services San Diego State University San Diego, California 3251 Riverport Lane St. Louis, Missouri 63043 COGNITION AND ACQUIRED LANGUAGE DISORDERS: ISBN: 978-0-323-07201-4 AN INFORMATION PROCESSING APPROACH Copyright © 2012 by Mosby, an imprint of Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. No part of this publication may be reproduced or transmitted in any form or by any means, electronic or mechanical, including photocopying, recording, or any information storage and retrieval system, without permission in writing from the publisher. Details on how to seek permission, further information about the Publisher’s permissions policies and our arrangements with organizations such as the Copyright Clearance Center and the Copyright Licensing Agency, can be found at our website: www.elsevier.com/permissions. This book and the individual contributions contained in it are protected under copyright by the Publisher (other than as may be noted herein). Notices Knowledge and best practice in this field are constantly changing. As new research and experience broaden our understanding, changes in research methods, professional practices, or medical treatment may become necessary. Practitioners and researchers must always rely on their own experience and knowledge in evaluating and using any information, methods, compounds, or experiments described herein. In using such information or methods they should be mindful of their own safety and the safety of others, including parties for whom they have a professional responsibility. With respect to any drug or pharmaceutical products identified, readers are advised to check the most current information provided (i) on procedures featured or (ii) by the manufacturer of each product to be administered, to verify the recommended dose or formula, the method and duration of administration, and contraindications. It is the responsibility of practitioners, relying on their own experience and knowledge of their patients, to make diagnoses, to determine dosages and the best treatment for each individual patient, and to take all appropriate safety precautions. To the fullest extent of the law, neither the Publisher nor the authors, contributors, or editors assume any liability for any injury and/or damage to persons or property as a matter of products liability, negligence or otherwise, or from any use or operation of any methods, products, instructions, or ideas contained in the material herein. ISBN: 978-0-323-07201-4 Vice President and Publisher: Linda Duncan Executive Editor: Kathy Falk Managing Editor: Jolynn Gower Publishing Services Manager: Julie Eddy Project Manager: Jan Waters Design Direction: Karen Pauls Printed in the United States of America Last digit is the print number: 9 8 7 6 5 4 3 2 1 Dedication To Patti and Kevin For reminding me every day what’s important Richard To Mitzi, Joelle, and Dillon My candles in the dark Lew CONTRIBUTORS Alfredo Ardila, PhD, ABPN Bruce Crosson, PhD Professor Department of Veterans Affairs Department of Communication Sciences Brain Rehabilitation Research Center of and Disorders Excellence Florida International University Malcolm Randall VA Medical Center Miami, Florida Department of Clinical & Health Psychology College of Public Health and Health Professions Kathleen Brumm, PhD pending University of Florida SDSU/UCSD Joint Doctoral Program Gainesville, Florida School of Speech, Language, and Hearing Sciences G. Albyn Davis, PhD San Diego State University Communicative Sciences and Disorders San Diego, California New York University New York, New York Michael Cannizzaro, PhD, CCC-SLP Assistant Professor Argye E. Hillis, MD, MA Graduate Program Coordinator Executive Vice Chair and Professor Department of Communication Sciences Co-director, Cerebrovascular Division and Disorders Department of Neurology The University of Vermont School of Medicine Burlington, Vermont Johns Hopkins University Baltimore, Maryland Thomas H. Carr, PhD, MS Professor Jacqueline J. Hinckley, PhD, CCC-SLP Department of Psychology Associate Professor Emeritus Michigan State University Department of Communication Sciences East Lansing, Michigan and Disorders University of South Florida Carl Coelho, PhD Tampa, Florida Professor and Department Head Communication Sciences Department Susan Kemper, PhD University of Connecticut Roberts Distinguished Professor Storrs, Connecticut Department of Psychology University of Kansas Matthew L. Cohen, MS Lawrence, Kansas Department of Clinical & Health Psychology Swathi Kiran, PhD, CCC-SLP College of Public Health and Health Associate Professor Professions Speech Language and Hearing Sciences University of Florida Sargent College of Health & Rehabilitation Gainesville, Florida Sciences Boston University Assistant in Neurology Massachusetts General Hospital Boston, Massachusetts v vi Contributors Tracy Love, PhD Liana S. Rosenthal, MD Professor Department of Neurology School of Speech, Language, and Hearing School of Medicine Sciences Johns Hopkins University San Diego State University Baltimore, Maryland Center for Research in Language University of California, San Diego Chaleece Sandberg, PhD Student San Diego, California Speech Language and Hearing Sciences Sargent College of Health & Rehabilitation Nadine Martin, PhD, CCC-SLP Sciences Professor Boston University Department of Communication Sciences Boston, Massachusetts and Disorders College of Health Professions and Social Work Lewis P. Shapiro, PhD Temple University Professor Philadelphia, Pennsylvania School of Speech, Language, and Hearing Sciences Randi Martin, PhD, MS College of Health and Human Services Elma Schneider Professor San Diego State University Department of Psychology San Diego, California Rice University Houston, Texas L. Robert Slevc, PhD Assistant Professor Richard K. Peach, PhD Department of Psychology Professor Program in Neuroscience and Cognitive Science Department of Communication Disorders University of Maryland and Sciences College Park, Maryland College of Health Sciences Rush University Leanne Togher, B App Sc (Speech Path), PhD Chicago, Illinois NHMRC Senior Research Fellow Associate Professor Josée Poirier, PhD Discipline of Speech Pathology Research Scientist The University of Sydney Language Processes Laboratory Sydney, Australia School of Speech, Language, and Hearing Sciences Julie A. Van Dyke, MSc, PhD San Diego State University Senior Research Scientist San Diego, California Haskins Laboratories New Haven, Connecticut PREFACE Clinical practice associated with acquired lan- of our higher mental processes. The study of human guage disorders has evolved in important ways language provides a unique way to understand human in the past 30 years as a result of advances in our nature, to do cognitive science, to dig deep into the understanding of the cognition of language. In 1982, science of mental life (Boeckx, 2010). This is the goal of the Committee on Language of the American Speech- psycholinguistics, the study of the mental processes and Language-Hearing Association (ASHA) defined language types of knowledge involved in understanding and as a rule-governed behavior that can be described by at producing language in both its oral and written forms. least five parameters—phonological, morphological, syn- Psycholinguistics examines “listening, speaking, read- tactic, semantic, and pragmatic—for which “learning ing, and writing, trying to discover the cognitive and use are determined by the interaction of biological, machinery and knowledge structures that underlie these cognitive, psychosocial, and environmental factors” skills and what role they play in linguistic behavior” (ASHA, 1983). The separation of language and cognition (De Groot, 2011, p. 2). in this definition was reinforced in a subsequent report In the case of language, impairments to such intrinsic by a subgroup of the same committee (the Subcommit- cognitive processes, whether they are attentional, memo- tee on Language and Cognition) to address the roles rial, linguistic, or executive, can produce language disor- of the speech-language pathologist in the habilitation ders. Breakdowns are not ones of processes that “interact” and rehabilitation of cognitively impaired individuals with language abilities. Rather, such breakdowns occur in (ASHA, 1987). In that report, cognition was described, processes that are fundamental to language itself. For this using Neisser’s (1967) definition, as “the processes by reason, we use the term acquired language disorders in this which sensory input is transformed, reduced, elaborated, text in lieu of the less desirable but well-entrenched term stored, recovered, and used” and was considered sepa- cognitive-communication impairments to emphasize the rately from language. In portraying “cognitive-language unity of cognition and language. From a cognitive neuro- relationships,” the report went on to list the “specific psychological perspective, acquired language disorders cognitive impairments that may affect language” by are but one example of the larger class of cognitive disor- contributing “to deficits in the semantic, syntactic, pho- ders (Lezak, Howieson, & Loring, 2004; Rapp, 2002). nologic, and/or pragmatic aspects of language.” The in- Identification of the processing impairments that con- dependence of the communication problems arising tribute to different types of acquired language disorders from these “cognitive” deficits from other types of lan- provides a basis for informed approaches to language guage disorders was emphasized and thus gave rise to the intervention and rehabilitation. category of so-called cognitive-communication impairments. Cognition and Acquired Language Disorders Cognitive-communication impairments were defined as is designed to be used as a primary textbook in graduate “communicative disorders that result from deficits in courses addressing the cognitive aspects of communica- linguistic and nonlinguistic cognitive processes” (p. 54). tion. Information is assembled in a consistent frame- The distinction between cognitive-communication and work composed of (1) normal cognitive processing for language disorders was further highlighted in the scope language in adults, (2) the cognitive impairments un- of practice for speech-language pathology (ASHA, 1990) derlying language disorders arising from a variety of with statements partitioning the practice for language neurological conditions, and (3) current assessment and versus cognitive-communication disorders. These dis- treatment strategies for the management of these disor- tinctions continue to be upheld in more recent updates ders. The text is organized using an information pro- of these clinical practice documents (ASHA, 2003, 2005, cessing approach to acquired language disorders and 2007). The descriptor cognitive-communication impair- thus can be set apart from more traditional syndrome- ments has evolved in some quarters into the even more based approaches (e.g., stroke, dementia, and traumatic problematic term cognitive-linguistic deficits. brain injury). In syndrome-based approaches, numerous These approaches suggest, as Davis (this volume) neurological conditions that produce acquired language explains, “that cognition plays a role in language and disorders (e.g., tumor, infection, degenerative diseases, communication, or that it is related to language and com- and multiple strokes) are often ignored. In the current munication, as if ‘language’ and ‘cognition’ are different processing approach, the language disorders that result things.” They are not. Language is part of cognition, part from a variety of neurological conditions are treated as vii viii Preface being more similar than the specific etiologies them- We want to express our thanks to the authors, all selves. As just one example, working memory and atten- experts in their chosen areas, for agreeing to contribute tion are considered “domain-general” operations that their work to this text. We are confident that the breadth, are disrupted in several types of disordered populations. depth, and overall excellence of their work will make this Similarly, the processing approach allows for the de- the most authoritative source available regarding cogni- scriptions and treatments to be applied across multiple tion and acquired language disorders. Finally, we would neurological groups who share specific cognitive deficits. like to thank Jolynn Gower, our managing editor at The chapters of this text describe how attentional, Elsevier, for providing outstanding support and guidance memorial, linguistic, and executive processes coalesce for the development of this text. We hope that you will in language functioning. The language characteristics find it to be a helpful resource for the clinical manage- of individuals presenting with a variety of neurological ment of acquired language disorders. conditions that impair these processes are also addressed, as well as the assessment and treatment of the resulting RKP language disorders with reference to the specific types of LPS underlying impairments. The intent is to provide an advanced discussion of this material for both graduate coursework in speech-language pathology and clinical REFERENCES neuropsychology and to offer a reference for practicing American Speech-Language-Hearing Association. (1983). Com- clinicians in these disciplines. mittee on Language: Definition of language. Asha, 24, 44. The text is divided into four sections. The first section American Speech-Language-Hearing Association. (1987). The provides an overview of cognition and language, as well role of speech-language pathologists in the habilitation and as tutorials describing the effects of aging on normal lan- rehabilitation of cognitively impaired individuals: A report guage processing and the neurological conditions that of the subcommittee on language and cognition. Asha, 29, are associated with acquired language disorders. The sec- 53–55. ond section provides an in-depth discussion of normal American Speech-Language-Hearing Association. (1990). Scope processing for attention, memory, language, and execu- of practice, speech-language pathology and audiology. Asha, 32 (Suppl. 2), 1–2. tive functioning and serves as a foundation for the subse- American Speech-Language-Hearing Association. (2003). Eval- quent discussion of language disorders. The third section uating and treating communication and cognitive disorders: examines the cognition of acquired language disorders, Approaches to referral and collaboration for speech-language and the fourth section provides guidance for the clinical pathology and clinical neuropsychology [Technical Report]. management of these disorders. Assessment and treat- Available from www.asha.org/policy. ment protocols that are provided are based on a review of American Speech-Language-Hearing Association. (2005). Roles current evidence so that students and clinicians will have of speech-language pathologists in the identification, diagnosis, a ready clinical resource for managing language disorders and treatment of individuals with cognitive-communication due to deficits in attention, memory, linguistic opera- disorders: Position Statement [Position Statement]. Available tions, and executive functions. from www.asha.org/policy. American Speech-Language-Hearing Association. (2007). Scope Following the introductory material, the text pro- of practice in speech-language pathology [Scope of Practice]. vides three chapters—one on normal processing, one on Available from www.asha.org/policy. disorder characteristics, and one on clinical approaches— Boeckx, C. (2010). Language in cognition: Uncovering mental struc- for each of the cognitive domains associated with lan- tures and the rules behind them. Chichester, West Sussex, UK: guage functioning and acquired language disorders. Wiley-Blackwell. Although each of the chapters of this text can be studied Davis, G. A. (2012). The cognition of language and communica- independently of the others, the structure of the text is tion. In R. K. Peach & L. P. Shapiro (Eds.), Cognition and acquired designed to encourage instructors to complete the read- language disorders (p. 1). St. Louis: Elsevier Mosby. ings for normal processing across all cognitive domains De Groot, M. B. A. (2011). Language and cognition in bilinguals and before proceeding to discussions of their applied coun- multilinguals: An introduction. New York: Psychology Press. Lezak, M. D., Howieson, D. B., & Loring, D. W. (2004). Neuropsy- terparts (i.e., disorders and interventions). This approach chological assessment (4th ed.). New York: Oxford University allows readers to fully appreciate the relations among Press. cognitive domains (e.g., attention and working mem- Neisser, U. (1967). Cognitive psychology. New York: Appleton, ory, working memory, and the central executive) Century, Cross. before proceeding to discussions of deficits within Rapp, B. (2002). The handbook of cognitive neuropsychology: What these domains in acquired language disorders due to deficits reveal about the human mind. Philadelphia: Psychology neurological impairments. Press. SECTION  IntroductIon ONE 1 CHAPTER  The Cognition of Language and Communication CHAPTER OUTLINE G. Albyn Davis Assumptions in the Study  Memory Executive Function of Cognition  Long-Term Memory Language and Communication Approaches to the Study  Working Memory A Closing Editorial of Cognition  Information Processing in Working Conclusions Attention Memory For a long time, language has had a curious relationship This first chapter introduces cognition and how to cognition in the vocabularies of rehabilitation practitio- it is studied, mainly in cognitive psychology. For ners, as well as laypersons. Diagnosticians have neatly the study of language processes, psycholinguists and divided and packaged disorders into separate categories. many speech-language pathologists use the methods “Language” has been viewed descriptively with assistance to be discussed. Then, the chapter provides an orienta- from linguistics (e.g., phonology, morphology, syntax), tion to later topics of attention, memory, executive whereas “cognition” has been identified broadly with function, and language. Subsequent chapters will be “intelligence” and specifically with mental functions more specific and expansive as to how cognition fuels such as attention, perception, and memory. In some language comprehension, formulation, and communi- quarters, morphology and memory have been considered cation. Mainly, the present chapter sets up the think- to be two separate entities, despite the reality that we ing behind the investigation of cognition. store morphology in our memory. To assess memory, we use tests of cognition; to assess morphology, we use tests ASSUMPTIONS IN THE STUDY  for aphasia. OF COGNITION It has been suggested that cognition plays a role in language and communication or that it is related to Cognition is “an umbrella term for all higher mental language and communication, as if “language” and processes . . . the collection of mental processes and “cognition” are different things. However, if cognition activities used in perceiving, remembering, thinking, is identified with information processing and we think and understanding” (Ashcraft & Radvansky, 2010, p. 9). of language use as information processing, then it is In contemplating their history, “cognitive psychologists consistent to think of language functions as embedded generally agree that the birth of cognitive psychology in cognition. Language comprehension and formula- should be listed as 1956” (Matlin, 2009, p. 7). Around tion are part of the cognitive system. When linguists this time, key publications and conferences steered characterize what we know about language, they are psychology away from behaviorism. This change was speaking of something in memory. driven by the Skinner-Chomsky debate over nurture 1 2 Chapter 1 n The Cognition of Language and Communication versus nature, George Miller’s measure of short-term imaging) was not matching the constructs for measure- memory as being around seven units, and interest at ment of mental operations. Now, with the emerging Carnegie-Mellon University in the computer as an anal- fine-tuned technologies of functional neuroscience ogy for human information processing. The shift was (Cabeza & Kingstone, 2006; Gazzaniga, Ivry, & Mangun, complete when the Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal 2008), current editions of texts on cognition include Behavior became the Journal of Memory and Language in chapters on the brain and sometimes are regaled with the early 1980s. Essentially, psychologists admitted that colorful pictures from brain imaging (e.g., Ashcraft mental processes exist. & Radvansky, 2010). Nevertheless, one can conduct This section introduces three of four assumptions experimental cognitive psychology without consider- underlying the study of mental processes. They are pre- ing the brain and, as a result, can restrict theory sented as a hierarchy of dichotomies in Figure 1-1. First, to functional matters (e.g., how memory works, as there is a working distinction between behavior (as evidence) opposed to how the brain works). and what happens in our heads (as theory). Similarly for In our everyday vocabularies, “brain” and “mind” clinical diagnosis, we consider the relationship of what often refer to the same thing. Yet, saying that someone we can observe (symptoms) to what we cannot observe has “lost his mind” does not mean that he has misplaced (diagnosed impairment). Scientists avoid writing state- his brain (Box 1-1). ments like “comprehension is a behavior” so that they In his text for speech-language pathologists, Davis do not think carelessly and confuse one for the other. (2007a) encouraged clear thinking by recommending Now that we are thinking inside the box, the second that we keep what happens to the brain (e.g., stroke, assumption differentiates the brain as a material thing from trauma) logically distinct from what happens to cogni- cognition as a mental thing. Because cognition is what tion (e.g., aphasia, amnesia). We can say that stroke the brain does and, therefore, is not truly independent causes aphasia (not that aphasia causes stroke). Neuro- of the brain, this dualism is largely a contrivance that is surgeons treat the brain, speech-language pathologists reflective of a research strategy. Cognitive psychologists treat cognition, and so on. Whether cognitive-language approached their work as if “the mind can be studied therapy re-wires the brain is a current question. At least, independently from the brain” (Johnson-Laird, 1983). to understand the nature of aphasia, we should have Through the 1980s, cognitive psychology texts barely some idea of what happens to cognition. mentioned the brain. At that time, Flanagan (1984) Putting aside the brain, the third assumption focuses stated that cognitive psychologists “by and large, sim- on cognition. Cognition consists of a fairly stable knowl- ply seem not to worry about the mind-brain problem.” edge base and fleeting processes. This distinction was This dualistic approach was necessary, because tech- helpful when clinical pioneer Hildred Schuell pro- nology for observing the brain (e.g., fuzzy structural claimed that what we do about aphasia depends on what we think aphasia is (Sies, 1974). A frequent ques- tion has been whether aphasia is an erasure of language Behavior Our heads knowledge or a disruption of language processing (while knowledge remains intact). The answer informs the broad approach to therapy, namely, whether it involves teaching words anew (because of a “loss” of Brain Cognition Box 1-1 Dialogue from a 1988 Episode of Miami Vice Knowledge Process Interrogator: What about the fact that he can’t remember any of his actions? Isn’t that a convenient lapse of memory? Physician: The answer to your question is that I’m a neurosurgeon. You’re questioning Detective Crockett’s Automatic Controlled mental capacities. That determination should be Figure 1-1  A hierarchy of increasingly specific assumptions made by a psychiatrist. underlying the study of cognition.

See more

The list of books you might like

Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.