ebook img

Burnout & Health Alex Chapter Shirom-Melamed PDF

48 Pages·2003·0.3 MB·English
Save to my drive
Quick download
Download
Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.

Preview Burnout & Health Alex Chapter Shirom-Melamed

Arie Shirom and Shmuel Melamed Burnout and health: Current Knowledge and Future Research Directions Burnout has received increased research attention in recent years. During the period of 1995-2002, annually about 150 articles that concerned burnout have appeared in journals covered by PsychInfo. As evident, burnout has been a major focus of researchers’ efforts. A recent review of the area of burnout (Schuafeli & Enzmann, 1998) found about 5,500 entries with burnout as a key word between 1975 and 1995. Notwithstanding this large number of studies, the relationships between burnout and physical health, including physiological risk factors and physical disease states, have hardly been explored. The literature on burnout and well being or mental health is substantial, but has not been reviewed with sufficient attention to the instruments used to gauge burnout and their respective construct validity. The objectives of this chapter are to review current knowledge on the above issues and to provide a perspective on future directions of research into burnout – health linkages. We start out by discussing the conceptual meaning of burnout. Burnout is viewed as an affective reaction to ongoing stress. We contend that core content of this affective reaction is the gradual depletion over time of individuals’ intrinsic energetic resources, leading to feelings of emotional exhaustion, physical fatigue, and cognitive weariness (Shirom, 1989). Given the multidimensionality of the construct and the controversy over its operational definition (Maslach, Schaufeli, & Leiter, 2001), this conceptual analysis is essential for understanding the possible health consequences of burnout. We seek in this chapter to propose mechanisms that link burnout and health (cf. Schaufeli & Greenglass, 2001). The following sections cover the empirical literature on burnout’s linkages with mental and physical health, respectively. Typically, empirical studies on the health consequences of burnout are based on a cross-sectional study design and measure burnout, and also mental health, by asking respondents to complete a self-report questionnaire. In this review, the emphasis is on longitudinal studies on burnout’s impact on health since they provide more credence to cause and effect statements. The voluminous empirical research on burnout has already been reviewed by several by meta-analytic studies (Collins, 1999; Lee & Ashforth, 1996; Schaufeli & Enzmann, 1998). Most of this research has measured burnout by the Maslach Burnout Inventory (MBI). In references made to this body of studies, we will focus on results reported for the emotional exhaustion scale. This review covers burnout of employees in work organizations, excluding research that deals exclusively with non-employment settings (e.g., athletes’ burnout: Dale & Weinberg, 1990). Also excluded is research that deals with burnout in life domains other than work, like crossover of burnout among marital partners (e.g., Pines, 1996; Westman & Etzion, 1995). The Conceptual Basis of Burnout During the 1980s and early 1990s, research on burnout, regardless of the conceptual approach employed, dealt almost exclusively with people-oriented professionals (e.g., teachers, nurses, doctors, social workers, and policepersons). People-oriented professionals often enter their mostly public sector profession with service-oriented idealistic goals. They typically work under norms that expect them to continuously invest emotional, cognitive and even physical energy in service recipients. In most of today’s advanced market economies, the public sector has to adjust to consumers’ growing demands for quality service, downsizing, and budgetary retrenchments. Inevitably, such a context of overloading and conflicting demands is a fertile ground for creating a process of emotional exhaustion, mental weariness and physical fatigue that Freudenberger, who pioneered in scientifically investigating this phenomenon, labeled as burnout. Freudenberger (1974, 1980) pioneering clinically oriented work on burnout inspired three different conceptual approaches to burnout, each with its distinct measure. We refer to the conceptual schemes and measures of Maslach and her colleagues (Maslach, 1982; Maslach & Leiter, 1997), of Pines and her colleagues (Pines & Aronson, 1988; Pines, Aronson & Kafry, 1981) and of Shirom and Melamed (Shirom, 1989; Hobfoll & Shirom, 1993, 2000; Melamed, Kushnir & Shirom, 1992). In this review, we will emphasize issues related to the validity of the first conceptual approach toward burnout, including the measurement instrument constructed by Maslach and her colleagues, the MBI (Maslach, Jackson, & Leiter, 1996). The reason for this focus is that the MBI was one of the very first scientifically validated burnout measurement instruments, and it has been the most widely used in scholarly research (Schaufeli & Enzmann, 1998). The first version of the MBI reflected the field’s preoccupation with professionals in people-oriented occupations. Subsequently, the construction of newer versions of the popular MBI, applicable to other occupational groups (Maslach et al., 1996) extended the study of burnout to other categories of employees. The Maslach Burnout Model and Inventory According to this conceptualization (Maslach & Jackson, 1981; Maslach, 1982; Maslach & Leiter, 1997), burnout is viewed as a syndrome that consists of three dimensions: emotional exhaustion, depersonalization, and reduced personal accomplishment. Emotional exhaustion, which refers to feelings of being depleted of one’s emotional resources, is regarded as the basic individual stress component of the syndrome (Maslach, Schaufeli, & Leiter, 2001). Depersonalization, referring to negative, cynical or excessively detached responses to other people at work, represents the interpersonal component of burnout. Reduced personal accomplishment, referring to feelings of decline in one’s competence and productivity and to one’s lowered sense of self-efficacy, represents the self-evaluation component of burnout (Maslach, 1998, p. 69). The three dimensions were not deducted theoretically but resulted from labeling exploratory factor-analyzed items initially collected to reflect the range of experiences associated with the phenomenon of burnout (Maslach, 1998, p. 68; Schaufeli & Enzmann, 1998, p. 51). Subsequently, Maslach and her colleagues modified the original definition of the latter two dimensions (cf. Maslach et al., 2001, p. 399). Depersonalization was renamed cynicism, though it still referred to the same cluster of symptoms involving other people at work rather than service recipients. However, the new label for this dimension of the syndrome poses new problems. As an emerging new concept in psychology and organizational behavior, the term cynicism is used to refer to negative attitudes involving frustration from, disillusionment with, and distrust of organizations, persons, groups or objects (Andersson & Bateman, 1997; Dean, Brandes, & Dharwadkar, 1998). Abraham (2000) has suggested that work cynicism, one of the forms of cynicism that she had identified in her research, tends to be closely related to burnout. Garden (1987) has argued that this dimension of the syndrome of burnout gauges several distinct attitudes, including distancing, hostility, rejection, and unconcern. It follows that the discriminant validity of this component of burnout, relative to the current conceptualizations of employee or work cynicism or relative to the other distinct attitudinal concepts noted by Garden (1987) has yet to be established. The third dimension was re-labeled as reduced efficacy or ineffectiveness, depicted to include the self-assessments of low self-efficacy, lack of accomplishment, lack of productivity, and incompetence (Leiter & Maslach, 2001). Each of these concepts, namely self-efficacy, accomplishment or achievement, personal productivity or performance, and personal competence, represents a distinct field of research in the behavioral sciences and the authors of the MBI have yet to clarify on what theoretical grounds they can be grouped together to represent a single conceptual entity. Does reduced efficacy refer to one’s confidence in one’s capability to successfully execute courses of action required to deal with prospective tasks, as self-efficacy is customarily defined (e.g., Lee & Bobko, 1994; Stajkovic & Luthans, 1998)? Does the third dimension of burnout reflect one’s belief in one’s knowledge and skills, as competence is often conceptualized (Foschi, 2000; Sandberg, 2000)? As an additional alternative, does it relate to self-assessed job performance or performance expectations (e.g., Stajkovic & Luthans, 1998)? It appears that each of the second and third dimensions of the MBI, as currently defined, probably represents each several multifaceted constructs, each having different theoretical implications with regard to the emotional exhaustion component of burnout suggested by the authors of the MBI (cf. Moore, 2000, p.341). Clearly, the conceptualization of burnout as tapped by the MBI relates to it as a multidimensional construct. A construct is multidimensional when it refers to several distinct but related dimensions that are viewed as a single theoretical construct (Law, Wong, & Mobley, 1998). The proponents of this multidimensional view of burnout (e.g., Maslach, 1998) argue that it provides a holistic representation of a complex phenomenon, broadly conceived as referring to the process of wear and tear or continuous encroachment upon employees’ resources. However, they have yet to provide convincing theoretical arguments as to why the three different clusters of symptoms that comprise their conceptualization of burnout should “hang together” (cf. Maslach et al., 2001). They further argue that their conceptualization allows researchers to use broadly conceived types of stress in both the work and the family domains as potential antecedents of burnout, thus increasing its explained variance. However, there is a paucity of evidence that there are specific antecedent variables or mechanisms leading to all of the three clusters of symptoms included in the MBI (Collins, 1999; Lee & Ashforth, 1996; Schaufeli & Enzmann, 1998). A case in point is the phase model of burnout, developed by Golembiewski and his colleagues and tested in a series of studies (see, for example, Golembiewski & Boss, 1992; Golembiewski, Munzenrider, & Stevenson, 1986; Golembiewski & Munzenrider, 1988). One of the theoretical assumptions upon which this model was based was that individuals experiencing burnout on the dimension of emotional exhaustion do not necessarily experience either of the other two clusters of symptoms. Indeed, Golembiewski and his colleagues (Golembiewski et al., 1986,1988, 1992) have provided the notion that each phase or dimension of burnout may develop independent of each other. Maslach (1998, p. 70) has argued that adding the dimensions of cynicism and reduced personal efficacy to the core dimension of emotional exhaustion was justified in that they add the interpersonal aspect of burnout to the conceptualization of the phenomenon. However, the emotional exhaustion scale of the MBI already includes items that tap interpersonal aspects of work, like “working with people all day is really a strain for me”, and “Working with people directly puts too much stress on me” (Maslach & Jackson, 1981). Conceptually, therefore, the view of burnout as a syndrome consisting of three clusters of symptoms lacks theoretical underpinnings, has not been supported by evidence demonstrating a common etiology for the three dimensions, and includes two clusters of symptoms, cynicism an reduced personal effectiveness, that appear to be too heterogeneous for advancing our knowledge on burnout. In sum, the MBI, the measurement scale whose process of construction has led inductively to the above conceptualization, has been the most popular instrument for measuring burnout in empirical research (for reviews of studies using it, see Collins, 1999; Lee & Ashforth, 1996; Schaufeli & Enzmann, 1998). It contained items purportedly assessing each of the three clusters of symptoms included in the syndrome view of burnout, namely emotional exhaustion, cynicism or depersonalization, and reduced effectiveness or lowered professional efficacy. It asks respondents to indicate the frequency over the work year with which they have experienced each feeling on a 7-point scale ranging from 0 (never) to 6 (every day). Three indices are usually constructed, relating to each of the above dimensions (for a recent psychometric critique, see Barnett, Brennan, & Careis, 1999). The factorial validity of the MBI has been extensively studied (Byrne, 1994; Handy, 1988; Lee & Ashforth, 1996; Schaufeli & Dierendonck, 1993; Schaufeli & Buunk, 1996). Most of the researchers examining this aspect of MBI validity have reported that a three-factor solution better fits their data than does a two-dimensional or a one-dimensional structure (for recent examples, see Boles, Dean, Ricks, Short & Wang, 2000; Schutte, Toppinen, Kalimo, & Schaufeli, 2000). Researchers using the MBI have most often constructed three different indices corresponding to the three dimensions of emotional exhaustion, cynicism, and reduced personal effectiveness. Several studies have argued, on both theoretical and psychometric grounds, that the use of a total score to represent total burnout should be avoided (e.g., Moore, 2000; Kalliath, O’Driscoll, Gillepsie & Bluedorn, 2000; Koeske & Koeske, 1989). The emotional exhaustion dimension has been consistently viewed as the core component of the MBI (e.g., Moore, 2000; Cordes, Dougherty & Blum, 1997; Burke & Greenglass, 1995). Most studies have shown it to be the most internally consistent and stable of the three components (Schaufeli & Enzmann, 1998). In meta-analytic reviews, it has been shown to be the most responsive to the nature and intensity of work-related stress (Lee & Ashforth, 1993; Schaufeli & Enzmann, 1998 Pines’ Burnout Model and Measure Pines and her colleagues define burnout as the state of physical, emotional and mental exhaustion caused by long –term involvement in emotionally demanding situations (Pines & Aronson, 1988, p. 9). This view does not restrict the application of the term burnout to the helping professions, as was initially the case with the first version of the MBI (Winnubst, 1993). Indeed, Pines and her colleagues have applied it not only to employment relationships (Pines, Aronson & Kafry, 1981) and organizational careers (Pines & Aronson, 1988), but also to marital relationships (Pines, 1988, 1996) and to the aftermath of political conflicts (Pines, 1993). Much like the case of the MBI, the Pines et al. conceptualization and measure of burnout, the BM (Burnout Measure) emerged from clinical experience and case studies. In the process of actually constructing the BM measure, Pines and her colleagues have moved away from their original conceptualization to an empirical definition that regards burnout as a syndrome of co-occurring symptoms that include helplessness, hopelessness, entrapment, decreased enthusiasm, irritability, and a sense of lowered self-esteem (cf. Pines, 1993). None of these symptoms is anchored in the context of work or employment relationships. The BM is considered a one-dimensional measure yielding a single composite burnout score. As we have noted, the overlap between the conceptual definition and the operational definition of the BM is minimal (cf. Schaufeli & Enzmann, p. 48). In addition, the discriminant validity of the BM, relative to depression, anxiety, and self-esteem, is in doubt (cf. Shirom & Ezrachi, 2003). This has led researchers to describe the BM as a general index of psychological distress that encompasses physical fatigue, emotional exhaustion, depression, anxiety, and reduced self-esteem (e.g., Schaufeli & Dierendonck, 1993, p. 645; Shirom & Ezrachi, 2003). Therefore, it appears irrelevant to assess the linkage between burnout and indicators of mental health like depression or anxiety using questionnaire measures, since the overlap between the items used to gauge burnout by the BM and depression or anxiety is considerable (Shirom & Ezrachi, 2003). As we were unable to find any evidence linking the BM to disease end states or to physiological risk factors for physical disease, we have not included studies that have used it in this review. Shirom-Melamed Burnout Model and Measure (SMBM) The conceptualization of burnout that underlies the Shirom-Melamed Burnout Measure (SMBM) was inspired by the work of Maslach and her colleagues and Pines and her colleagues, and views burnout as an affective state characterized by one’s feelings of being depleted of one’s physical, emotional, and cognitive energies. Burnout follows prolonged exposure to chronic stress. Relative to chronic stress, event-based conceptualizations of stress, like those that relate to critical life events or acute stress and to episodic stress or hassles, derive from different theoretical approached (Derogatis & Coons, 1993) and has been found to be differently related to physiological risk factors in coronary heart disease (Kahn & Byosiere, 1992). Theoretically, the SMBM was based on Hobfoll’s (1989, 1998) Conservation of Resources [COR] theory. COR theory’s fundamental tenets are that people have a basic motivation to obtain, retain, and protect that which they value, including material, social, and energetic resources. According to COR theory (Hobfoll, 1989, 1998) stress at work occurs when individuals are either threatened with resource loss, lose resources, or fail to regain resources following resource investment. One of the corollaries of COR theory is that stress does not occur as a single event, but rather represents an unfolding process, wherein those who lack a strong resource pool are more likely to experience cycles of resource loss. The affective state of burnout is likely to exist when individuals experience a cycle of resource loss over a period of time at work (Hobfoll & Freedy, 1993). For example, a reference librarian who comes to work every morning to face yet another line of students impatiently awaiting her help, lacking opportunities to replenish her resources, is likely to cycle into a forceful spiral of resource loss and as a result feel burned out at work. The conceptualization of burnout formulated by Shirom (1989) based on COR theory (Hobfoll & Shirom, 1993, 2000) relates to energetic resources only, and covers physical, emotional, and cognitive energies. Burnout thus represents a combination of physical fatigue, emotional exhaustion, and cognitive weariness, three closely interrelated factors (Hobfoll & Shirom, 2000) that can be represented by a single score of burnout. Physical fatigue refers to feelings of tiredness and low levels of energy in carrying out daily tasks at work, like getting up in the morning to go to work. Emotional exhaustion refers to feeling too weak to display empathy to clients or coworkers and lacking the energy needed to invest in relationships with other people at work. Cognitive weariness refers to feelings of slow thinking and reduced mental agility. Each component of burnout covers the draining and depletion of energetic resources in a particular domain. There are three reasons for the focus on the combination of physical fatigue, emotional exhaustion and cognitive weariness in the conceptualization of burnout that has led to the construction of the SMBM. First, these forms of energy are individually possessed, and theoretically are expected to be closely interrelated. COR theory postulates that personal resources affect each other and exist as a resource pool, and that lacking one is often associated with lacking the other (Hobfoll & Shirom, 2000). Empirical research conducted with the SMBM has supported the linkage among physical fatigue, emotional exhaustion and cognitive weariness (e.g., Melamed, Kushnir & Shirom, 1992; Shirom, Westman, Shamai, & Carel, 1997). Second, the three forms of individually possessed energy included in the SMBM represent a coherent set that does not overlap any other established behavioral science concept, like depression and anxiety or like aspects of the self-concept such as self- esteem and self-efficacy. Third, the conceptualization of the SMBM clearly differentiates burnout from stress appraisals anteceding burnout, from coping behaviors that individuals may engage in to ameliorate the negative aspects of burnout like distancing themselves from

Description:
This review covers burnout of employees in work organizations, excluding research often enter their mostly public sector profession with service-oriented . Much like the case of the MBI, the Pines et al. conceptualization and motivation to obtain, retain, and protect that which they value, includ
See more

The list of books you might like

Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.