ebook img

Brain Mechanisms and Spatial Vision PDF

468 Pages·1985·29.779 MB·English
Save to my drive
Quick download
Download
Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.

Preview Brain Mechanisms and Spatial Vision

Brain Mechanisms and Spatial Vision NATO ASI Series Advanced Science Institutes Series A Series presenting the results of activities sponsored by the NATO Science Committee, which aims at the dissemination of advanced scientific and technological knowledge, with a view to strengthening links between scientific communities. The Series is published by an international board of publishers in conjunction with the NATO Scientific Affairs Division A Life Sciences Plenum Publishing Corporation B Physics London and New York C Mathematical and D. Reidel Publishing Company Physical Sciences Dordrecht and Boston D Behavioural and Martinus Nijhoff Publishers Social Sciences DordrechtlBoston/Lancaster E Applied Sciences F Computer and Springer-Verlag Systems Sciences BerlinlHeidelberglNew York G Ecological Sciences Series D: Behavioural and Social Sciences - No. 21 Brain Mechanisms and Spatial Vision edited by David J. Ingle The Rowland Institute for Science Cambridge, MA 02142 USA Marc Jeannerod Laboratoire de Neuropsychologie Experimentale INSERM - Unite 94 Bron, France David N. Lee Department of Psychology University of Edinburgh Edinburgh, UK 1985 Martinus Nijhoff Publishers Dordrecht / Boston / Lancaster Published in cooperation with NATO Scientific Affairs Division Proceedings of the NATO Advanced Study Institute on Brain Mechanisms and Spatial Vision, Lyon, France, June 16-25, 1983 Library of Congress Catalog Card Number: 84-27297 ISBN-13: 978-94-010-8743-8 e-I SBN-13: 978-94-009-5071-9 001: 10.1007/978-94-009-5071-9 Distributors for the United States and Canada: Kluwer Boston, Inc., 190 Old Derby Street, Hingham, MA 02043, USA Distributors for the UK and Ireland: Kluwer Academic Publishers, MTP Press Ltd, Falcon House, Queen Square, Lancaster LA1 1RN, UK Distributors for all other countries: Kluwer Academic Publishers Group, Distribution Center, P.O. Box 322, 3300 AH Dordrecht, The Netherlands All rights reserved. No part of this publication may be reproduced, stored in a retrieval system, or transmitted, in any form or by any means, mechanical, photocopying, recording, or otherwise, without the prior written permission of the publishers, Martinus Nijhoff Publishers, P.O. Box 163, 3300 AD Dordrecht, The Netherlands Copyright © 1985 by Martinus Nijhoff Publishers, Dordrecht Softcover reprint of the hardcover 1s t edition 1985 v Preface This volume contains chapters derived from a N.A.T.O. Advanced Study Institute held in June 1983. As the director of this A.S.I. it was my hope that some of the e1ectrophysiologists could express the potentialities of their work for perceptual theory, and that some perceptionists could speculate on the underlying "units" of perception in a way that would engage the imagination of physio logists. The reader will have to be the judge of whether this was achieved, or whether such a psychophysiological inter1ingua is still overly idealistic. It is clear that after the revolution prec~pitated by Hube1 and Weisel in understanding of visual cortical neurons we still have only a foggy idea of the behavioral output of any particular species of cortical detector. It was therefore particularly unfortunate that two persons who have made great strides in correlating interesting facets of cat cortical physio logy with human psychophysics (Max Cynader and Martin Regan of Dalhousie University) were unable to attend this meeting. Never theless, a number of new and challenging ideas regarding both spatial perception and cortical mechanisms are represented in this volume, and it is hoped that the reader will remember not only the individual demonstrations but the critical questions posed by the apposition of the two different collections of experimental facts. David Ingle April 1984 VII TABLE OF CONTENTS PREFACE V D.N. Lee and D.S. Young Visual Timing of Interceptive Action 1 J.J. Koenderink Space, Form and Optical Deformations 31 K. Nakayama Extraction of Higher Order Derivatives of the Optical Velocity Vector Field: Limitations Imposed by Biological Hardware 59 J. Todd The Analysis of Three-Dimensional Structure from Moving Images 73 B. Rogers and M. Graham Motion Parallax and the Perception of Three-Dimensional Surfaces 95 A. Yonas and C.E. Granrud The Development of Sensitivity to Kinetic, Binocular and Pictoral Depth Information in Human Infants 113 B. Timney Visual Experience and the Development of Depth Perception 147 J.R. Lackner Human Sensory-Motor Adaption to the Terrestrial Force Environment 175 R. Schmid, A. Buizza and D. Zambarbieri Visual Stabilization During Head Movement 211 D.J: Ingle and B.L. Shook Action-Oriented Approaches to Visuo-Spatial Brain Functions 229 VIII A. Cowey Disturbances of Stereopsis by Brain Damage 259 M. Jeannerod The Posterior Parietal Area as a Spatial Generator 279 J. Paillard and B. Amblard Static Versus Kinetic Visual Cues for the Processing of Spatial Relationships 299 H. Kennedy and G. Orban Behavioural and Neurophysiological Correlates of Visual Movement Deprivation in the Cat 331 G.A. Orban Velocity Tuned Cortical Cells and Human Velocity Discrimination 371 P. Hammond Visual Cortical Processing: Texture Sensitivity and Relative Motion 389 B.J. Frost Neural Mechanisms for Detecting Object Motion and Figure Ground Boundaries~ Contrasted with Self-Motion Detecting Systems 415 G. Jansson Perceptual Theory and Sensory Substitution 451 INDEX 467 VISUAL TIMING OF INTERCEPTIVE ACTIGN David N. Lee and David S. Y0ung Department. of Psycho10gy University of Edinburgh Edinburgh EH8 9JZ Scotland 1 . INTRODUCTION One of the most remarkable aspects of motor skill is the precision with which actions can be timed. In this chapter we will be concerned with the control of actions whose timing is dictated by how the organism is moving relative to the environment, as in locomotion, or by how an object is moving relative to the organism, as in catching or hitting something. We will refer to this as extrinsic timing. It requires predictive information about the relative motion of the organism and objects and surfaces in the environment, information that is often only available through VlSlon. Time to contact (meaning, in general, time to nearest approach) is a particularly important predictor. Consider, for example, catching a moving object with one hand an ability which starts developing as early as 18 weeks of age in human infants (1). In catching a ball not only has the hand to be positioned, properly oriented, on the trajectory of the ball but also the fingers have to start closing just before the ball arrives. If hand closure starts too late, the ball will bounce off the palm too early and it will rap the knuckles. With a moderate speed ball, timing has to be accurate to about 25ms (2), whereas in fast ball games like cricket the required precision is probably around 10ms. The timing of the catch is clearly under visual control, but this is not to say that the ball needs to be watched all the way to the hand: providing it is visible about 200-300ms before contact, reasonably precise timing can be achieved (3, 4). Nonetheless, sight of the ball over the last 2 lOOns to contact can significantly improve performance (5). It would appear, therefore, that the timing of a catch is normally under continuous visual control, with the visual information ~etting progressively more precise as contact is approached. A particularly dramatic example of extrinsic timing in locomotion is ski jumping. After accelerating to around 60mph down the steep in-run in crouched posture skiers have to forcefully straighten up just before they reach the lip of the in-run in order to launch themselves. Very precise timing is required. In a film analysis of 14 Olympic ski-jumpers it was found that on average they started straightening when their time to contact with the lip was 194ms and the standard deviation of this time interval across the 14 jumpers was a mere 10ms (6). The ability to time interceptive acts rapidly and precisely, as shown in these examples and in many other sports as well as animal activities (e.g. Ingle, this volume) suggests that visuo-motor systems have evolved which are particularly efficient at detecting time to contact and gearing actions to the infor~ation. In seeking to understand how the systems might work, it is logical to start with an examination of the optical input. This we do in the following section, showing how time to contact under constant approach velocity is specified optically. The perception of time to contact does not, in principle, require perceiving distance and speed. In Section 3, we extend the theory to how actions are timed when approach velocity is not constant, as when something is falling. Section 4 describes an experiment testing the theory. Applications of the ideas to the problem of motor control in hemiparetics and to the training of young children in crossing the road are presented in Sections 5 and 6. In Section 7 the theory is extended to visual control of locomotor acts and Sections 8 and 9 report related experiments. 2. TIME TO CONTACT AND THE TAU-MARGIN As we have said, organisms need predictive information in order to time their actions. Consider a person about to catch a ball. His actions might be driven by a complex predictive system which takes into account any available information about the ball's current position, velocity, acceleration, rate of change of acceleration, and so on. However, an over-complex system could entail unnecessary delays in making use of the information as well as over-sensitivity to nOise, and as such would be unlikely to have evolved. A better system is robust rather than over-refined and takes advantage of the most reliable and rapidly available information. 3 Ball Point of observation t D - ! -r- Figure 1. How the tau-margin is optically specified for a ball, diaDeter D, on a collision course. At time t it is distance r from the point of observation. By definition, the tau-Dargin = -r/}. (The dot denotes rate of change with tiDe.) The solid angle A = rr D2/4r2; differentiating, A = -1rD2r/2rl. Dividing these equations sives 2A/A = -r/0 = the tau-Dargin. The physical quantity that we propose as an estimate of the time to contact of an object heading for an organism is its cHst30ce away divided by its speed of approach. This would be an exact estimate of the time to contact if the relative acceleration of the object and organism were zero right up to contact. We call it the tau-margin, after the optic variable T which specifies it. We emphasize that the tau-margin does not have to be obtained from a distance and a velocity through a division; that is merely a convenient way to define it. Indeed, the physical relationship of the object and organism at a moment in time can be described with the tau-margin as a primitive quantity and distance and velocity as derived quantities. We suggest that an organism trying to intercept a moving object exploits such a description. Since an object to be intercepted is not always on a collision course with an organism's point of observation (e.g., when playing tennis) we define the tau-margin in general to be the time to the nearest approach of the object to the pOint of observation if the relative velocity of the two were to remain constant. To specify the relative velocity requires a frame of reference, a set of directions that are fixed relative to each other. Furthermore, in order that the tau-margin is defined the frame of reference must not rotate relative to the environment. If, for example, it were locked to the organism's eye then the relative velocity and hence the tau-margin - would change in value whenever the eye rotated. The reference frame may however move without rotation with the organism through the environment. In what way is the tau-margin, which partially describes the spatio-temporal relationship of the ball to the catcher at any moment in its flight, useful for controlling action? If the

See more

The list of books you might like

Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.