BOO! SERIES IN AFFECTIVE SCIENCE SERIES EDITORS Richard J. Davidson Paul Ekman Klaus Scherer Boo! Culture, Experience, and the Startle Reflex Ronald C. Simons New York Oxford OXFORD UNIVERSITY PRESS 1996 Oxford University Press Oxford New York Athens Auckland Bangkok Bogota Bombay Buenos Aires Calcutta Cape Town Dar es Salaam Delhi Florence Hong Kong Istanbul Karachi Kuala Lumpur Madras Madrid Melbourne Mexico City Nairobi Paris Singapore Taipei Tokyo Toronto and associated companies in Berlin Ibadan Copyright © 1996 by Oxford University Press, Inc. Published by Oxford University Press, Inc., 198 Madison Avenue, New York, New York 10016 Oxford is a registered trademark of Oxford University Press All rights reserved. No part of this publication may be reproduced, stored in a retrieval system, or transmitted, in any form or by any means, electronic, mechanical, photocopying, recording, or otherwise, without the prior permission of Oxford University Press. To protect privacy, informants' names and other potentially identifying information have been altered throughout except where permission for their use was explicitly granted. "Startle Neurosis" by EC. Thorne, American Journal of Psychiatry, 101,105-109. Copyright (c) 1944/45. the American Psychiatric Association. Reprinted by permission. "Lanti, illness by fright among Bisayan Filipinos." In R.C. Simons and C.C. Hughes (Eds.), The Culture-Bound Syndromes (pp. 371-398). Copyright (c) D. Reidel. Reprinted by permission of Kluwer Academic Publishers. An Introduction to Haiku by Harold G. Henderson. Copyright (c) 1958 by Harold G. Henderson. Used by permission of Doubleday, a division of Bantam Doubleday Dell Publishing Group, Inc. On Love and Barley: Haiku of Basho, translated and edited by Lucien Stryke. Copyright University of Hawaii Press. Used with permission. Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data Simons, Ronald C. Boo! : culture, experience, and the startle reflex / Ronald C. Simons. p. cm.— (Series in affective science) Includes index. ISBN 0-19-509626-6 1. Startle reaction—Social aspects. 2. Startle reactions—Cross-cultural studies. 3. Latah (Disease)—Cross-cultural studies. I. Title. II. Series. QP372.6.S57 1995 95-11809 304.5—dc20 9 8 7 6 5 4 3 2 1 Printed in the United States of America on acid free paper For Sherwood L. Washburn This page intentionally left blank PREFACE An analysis of the biology of the actors is essential to the understanding of any human social system. Sherwood Washburn & Elizabeth McCown, 1980, Human Evolution: Biosocial Perspectives, p. 7 Accepting that humans are a product of evolution, we must endeavor to understand how the behavioral propensities with which human individuals have been endowed by natural selection can give rise to the complexities of human relationships and of societies, and how societal forces shape the nature of the individual.... Certainly it is here... that the really exciting challenge now lies. Robert Hinde, 19S9, Animal Behavior, 38, p. 732 The work that this book reports began in 1975, when I spent a sabbatical year in the anthropology department of the University of California at Berkeley as a graduate student in cultural anthropology and simultane- ously as a postdoctoral student in physical anthropology with Professor Sherwood Washburn. It was at Berkeley that I began the study of the latah reaction, a "culture-bound syndrome" of Malaysia and Indonesia. As latah is discussed in some detail in the body of this book it is not necessary to describe it here, only to say that it is a set of rather peculiar behaviors per- formed by some individuals after they are startled. What made studying latah so intriguing is the fact that, although it is generally regarded as merely a Malay and Indonesian custom, and therefore a cultural artifact, a similar set of behaviors occurs in a most improbable set of other places around the globe—Siberia, Maine, Yemen, and the Island of Hokkaido, to name a few. No one had suggested any plausible explana- tion why. I wondered if the answer might have anything to do with the biol- ogy of the startle reflex, but if so, why did it occur only in scattered locations viii Preface and not ubiquitously? Much of the empirical work I was to do concerned itself with attempting to solve this puzzle. As E M. Yap had suggested in his seminal 1952 paper on latah, the solution seemed to be the result of some interaction among the neurophysiology of startle, what people thought about startle, and how these ideas were reflected in social action. In the course of the work on latah, I became interested in what happens when peo- ple are startled in other contexts and in the larger question of how this sim- ple evolved reflex enters into the private and social lives of people more gen- erally. How is it that the same bit of neurophysiology is played out and experienced so similarly and yet so differently in different cultural contexts? Thus startle, rather than latah per se, gradually became the focus of the work. The startle reflex—ubiquitous, discrete, and brief—presented itself as a Rosetta stone. I could look both at what people did with this simple reflex and also at what they said about it in a wide set of contexts, in both the popular and scholarly literatures. Just as Carlyle's Sartor Resartus had humorously examined human life as reflected in clothing, I could look at experience, interaction, and explanation as they were reflected in startle, using startle as a model system. Different disciplines developed different data, but data that could in this case be reconciled from one discipline to another along the dimensions of the startle reflex. As I began to publish on this work, it was my good fortune to have an especially fluent and persuasive intellectual adversary, Michael Kenny, of Simon Fraser University. Professor Kenny took on the task of opposing the position I present here in a series of papers in which the relevant issues were argued back and forth between us. The opportunity to participate in this debate enormously clarified for me the line of thinking presented in this text. Much of the material in the second part of this book is an expansion of materials and ideas first developed in the course of that debate. The debate appeared in the pages of the Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, which under the editorship of Professor Eugene Brody has long been a pub- lication that encourages the thoughtful presentation of more than one side of any issue. To the Journal and to Professor Brody I am much indebted. While this book was in press, Robert L. Winzeler published a work sum- marizing his long study of latah. In many ways our observations about the previous literature on latah and about the cultural and social setting in which contemporary latah is embedded are similar and mutually reinforcing. How- ever, starting from rather disparate premises about the relationship of cul- ture to biology, we arrive at quite different conclusions about the nature of latah, why latah has the form and cultural patterning we see. Our conclusions about latah differ in two important respects: the significance we find in the similarities among the family of culture-specific "latah" syndromes and, most importantly, the significance of neurophysiological factors in the syn- drome's contemporary Malaysian presentations. To anyone interested in fur- ther pursuing the study of latah, Winzeler's book can be highly recommended. Organizing and explaining the heterogenous assemblage of materials that have been collected over the years presented some major expository problems. Initially, I began with a discussion of latah which, after all, was Preface ix the original subject of research. However, after wrestling with the mater- ial unprofitably for rather too long, I realized that most readers would find it easier to move from the familiar to the unfamiliar. Hence the organiza- tion of this book, with the section on latah and other non-Western mani- festations of startle following material closer to the lives of most Western readers. The major problem proved to be that of sequentially presenting data and concepts linked by an intricate network of connections. Of all the relevant disciplines, it is anthropology with its cultural, physical, linguistic, and archaeologic branches that makes the strongest claim to being a unified science of humankind. Yet in actuality, this is more of an idealized goal than a usual practice. Paul J. Bohannan once expressed the tension between this ethos and practice in an open letter to the mem- bership of the American Anthropological Society published almost two decades ago on the front page of the Anthropology Newsletter: Anthropology covers a wider range than any other social science: from human biology to human language, from prehistory to civi- lization. That broad scope is not an accident or a mirror of vanity. To carry out our main job—determining the many ways that the many dimensions of human life can be fitted together—it has to be broad. Anthropology is an integrated and synthesizing disci- pline: it is within itself necessarily interdisciplinary. Its primary mode is one of comparison.... The need for precise monographic study of particular behav- ioral situations is obvious: without them we could not be compar- ative [However], the comparative task—which is, in my opin- ion, the primary one—seems far more difficult. Perhaps because it is farther removed from artifacts and from people. And as time goes on and a subject develops, it becomes increasingly difficult to make the leap to the comparative and the general. Yet, the fact of the broadness and the difficulty of making that leap gives anthropology the appearance of being constantly on the verge of shattering into its components. The unifying cement is something that must be constantly worked on if we are to preserve it. (Bohannan, 1978, pp. 1-2) It is my hope that this book will function as a bit of the necessary cement. It is a book of connections. It is also my hope that it will be of use to those who hyperstartle and who find their hyperstartling problematic, and to those who will follow in the cross-cultural study of startle, hyper- startle, latah, and other culture-bound syndromes. I am much indebted to the Harry Frank Guggenheim Foundation, which supplied the major portion of the costs of my fieldwork in Southeast Asia; to the University of California International Center for Medical Research (Grant AI 10051 from the National Institutes of Health, United States Public Health Service); and to the Institut Penyelidikan Perubatan, Kuala Lumpur, Malaysia, which provided office space and much logistic support during the time I was in the field.
Description: