ebook img

Avoidance Behavior in Conflict Situations PDF

98 Pages·04.924 MB·English
Save to my drive
Quick download
Download
Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.

Preview Avoidance Behavior in Conflict Situations

This dissertation has been microfilmed exactly as received 67-11,381 FRENCH, Elizabeth Godbeer, 1916- AVOIDANCE BEHAVIOR IN CONFLICT SITUATIONS. Yale University, Ph.D„ 1942 Psychology, experimental University Microfilms, Inc., Ann Arbor, Michigan Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. © Copyright by ELIZABETH GODBZZR FRENCH 1967 Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Avoidance Behavior in Conflict Situations tgr Elizabeth Godbeer French An essay presented to the Faculty and the Graduate School of iale University in candidacy for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Digest The present investigation was an attempt to determine under lhat conditions going-out-of-the-fleld or escape responses occur in conflict situations. Two types of conflict, the double approach-avoidance in vbich there is instigation to approach and avoid each of two goals, and the single approach-avoidance in which there is instigation to approach and avoid a single goal, were studied. The experimental groups were established in the following way. The subjects were divided into six matched groups on the basis of their behavior in a pre­ liminary observation period. Then, in the firs t p art of the experimental period, a mildly pleasant affect for two tasks was artificially induced in the subjects of two of the groups, a neutral affect for the same tasks in the subjects of two groups, and an unpleasant affect for the tasks in the subjects of two groups. When th is had been accomplished, one group possessing each type of affect was instructed to select one of the tasks and work longer on it, and the other group of each lype was told which one to do. Thus double approach-avoidance conflicts in which the instigation was prim arily approach, in which i t was equal, and in which i t was prim arily avoidance were estab­ lished in three of the groups. Similar groups were established for the single approach-avoidance type. After the subjects had been instructed as to th eir further work they were told that the experimenter would have to be gone from the room for a period of time twice as long as was required for the completion of the task but that they were to ilnish i t and wait for her. Their behavior during th is period was observed through a one way vision mirror and recorded at five second intervals. In the room during the entire period were, as w ell-as the tasks, several games placed apparently at random, which were intended to serve as instigators extrinsic to the conflict situar- tion. The results showed that the subjects who had to choose between two un­ pleasant alternatives made the greatest response to the extrinsic objects before they began a task; those who had to do one unpleasant task showed the next greatest; those who had to do one neutral, next; those who had to choose between two neutral, next; and those who had only pleasant affect made no such responses. The amount of oscillation, m otility, and blocking at the moment of decision were found to increase in the same order. Except for the order of the two neutral groups, these results were in accord with the predictions, based on the stimulus response principles, that the amount of response to the extrinsic instigator and other manifestations of behavior disruption would increase writh the number and relative strength of the avoidance instigations present. The reversal Is^discussed in terms of the affect present. It was further demonstrated that the oscillation, m otility and blocking occurred prim arily just before a choice was made and at the time the approach instigation was becoming stronger due to the nearness of the time lim it. The possibility of generalizing from th is experiment to life situations is disc vis sed. The behavior to be expected after a task was selected and some work had been done on it is discussed in terms of the original affect for the task and the probable effect of the behavior that affect had caused. It was found that the stimulus response principles, which took into account the fact that be­ havior is a sequence and not a series of isolated events, were able to pre­ dict accurately what this subsequent behavior would be. It was expected that the subject with strong instigation to avoid at the beginning would have equally strong instigation to avoid a fte r a task was started bub that his Initial delay,caused by the strong avoidance,would not leave him time enough to leave th task again. Those who had mild instigation to avoid,on the other hand,would star soon enough so th at they would be able to leave the task from time to time snd s till finish before the lim it. This was what actually occurred.The possibilities of prediction from the stimulus response principles and from the field theoreti­ cal system of Lewin are compared. Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Acknowledgment The w riter wishe s to acknowledge her indebtedness to Professor Robert R. Sears for his aid during the formulation of the problem and the course of the investigation as well as his helpful criticism of the manuscript. Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Contents Digest 1 Introduction The Present Problem 7 Procedure 19 Measurement 27 28 Results Discussion 53 i Appendix i A. Individual data B. Summary of the stimulus response work on conflict x iii Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Avoidance Behavior in Conflict Situations w* "*■ I Introduction This investigation is an attempt to analyse experimentally the escape, or as Lewin (1935) describes it, the "going out of the field" response that occurs in certain types of conflict situations. That such responses do occur, or w ill unless specifically prevented, in conflicts where avoidance instigation is sufficiently strong, has been recognised by many experimenters. Since they desired to study the re­ solution of the conflict they took some pains to prevent the occurrence of such behavior. Thus knowledge of the subject is for the most part based on casual observations and such theoretical discussions as that of Lewin. Theoretically, according to Lewin, "going out of the field" should not occur where the conflict situation involves two positive goals, but i t may be expected when the choice is between two negative goals, or when the individual wishes to approach some goal but is afraid or prevented from doing so by some barrier. In the case of the child ifco has to decide whether to go on a picnic or play with his friends (Type 1 co n flict> a ll vectors are diagrammed as pointing in the direction of the goal and a stable equilibrium exists at the point of choice. He diagrams the situation of the child #10 wants to fetch a rubber swan from the water bub is afraid (Type 2 conflict) as consisting of a convergent fie ld corresponding to the positive valence of the swan, overlaid by a second fieLd vtiich corresponds to the negative valence of Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. 2 the water. According to the diagram the child must more to-the point of equilibrium between the two field s. This point w ill approach and retreat from the water according to the momentary oscillations of the situation, and such oscillations w ill be reflected in the behavior of the child. Leaving the fie ld occurs after repeated attempts to overcome the barrier. This is usually not permanent at firs t, but after repeated returns the child w ill leave not to return again. A third type of conflict is exenplified by the case of the child forced to do a disagreeable task under the threat of punishment. Here, since the vectors point away from th eir respective goals, the child is forced out of the fie ld once he leaves the point of equilibrium. Thus he w ill always go out of the field if not prevented. This type of behavior may be either physical, actually leaving the situation bodLly, or i t may be psychological in th at the child occupies himself with something else or "encysts". Lewin discusses going out of the field from a practical point of view in his chapter on reward and punishment (1935). He points out, for example, that parents who use the threat of punishment to force their children to perform a hated task mast be careful to erect a barrier around the total situation if the threat of punishment is to be effective. He mentions running away, concealing from the adult the fact that some other circumstance made the completion of the task unnecessary, and fantasying completion, as possible means used by the child to leave the field. The cases he describes involve a ll of these methods in various combinations. If the barrier is very firm and the conflict very severe, there may be no alternative^ le ft for the child but suicide. Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Following the approach of Lewin, Dernbo (1931) found that in her experiment on anger, wheih a ll the real solutions had been ex­ hausted, the subjects tended to resort to fantasy solutions. Fajans (1933) found in a success and failure experiment, that consolation or a substitute for real success very often was as effective as actual success far the young child. A1 alternative approach to the field theoretical one, and one that w ill possibly explain more of the data and in more detail, is the stimulus response interpretation. Conflict behavior has been analysed theoretically and observed experimentally from this point of view by M iller (1937, 1942), Brown (1940, 1942), Hovland and Sears (1938,1941), and others, who have secured a certain amount of evidence relevant to the present problem.^ A conflict situation is defined as one in which instigation to make incompatible responses is present. These may be approach responses, i.e ., going toward a goal or performing an act; or they may be avoidance responses, i.e ., running away from a given point or attempting to escape performing a given act. Instigation to these two types of responses may appear in various combinations, and ■various types of conflict are distinguished on the basis of these combinations. Hovland and Sears (1938) have distinguished four types of conflict on th is basis, the firs t three of fhich correspond with Lewin’s types mentioned above. In Type 1, described as an approach-approach conflict, there is simultaneous instigation to two incompatitie^approach responses. In Type 2 (approach-avoidance) there is instigation to 1. This work is summarized in the appendix. The findings perepent t* the present problem of going out of the field w ill be prejgjpMted^here, as w ell as an experiment by Brown (1942) in which he actually dealt with the problem. Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. I approach and avoid a singl e goal* In Type 3 (avoidance-avoidance) ., two inconpatible avcd. dance reaponaea are aimultaneou sly inetigated* The fourth type, Which is not mentioned by Lewin, ia a combination of the previous types. This occurs when there is both approach and avoidance instigation to each of two incompatible responses. This type is called a double approach-avoidance, or a Type 4 conflict* Of these four types, only two commonly occur in uncomplicated form in human subjects. These are the single approach-avoidance and the double approach-avoidance. Those which appear on the surface to be pure approach-approach or avoi dance-avoidanc e types are usually found, on analysis, to be cases of double approach-avoidance. (Hovland and Sears, 1938, Godbeer, 1940, M iller, 1942). For practical purposes, then, there are two types. The double approach-avoidance may vary in its resemblance to the pure approach-approach or the pure avoidance- avoidance type depending on the relative strength of the instigations present* In Type 1 conflict, or Type 4 conflict th at closely resembles it, no behavior disruption was expected or found (H&viand'aqd3Se&rs, 1938, Klebanoff, 1939)* When avcidance instigation becomes a little stronger, some hesitation appears (Godbeer, 1940), but a choice may s till be made* When the avoidance instigation is equal to that of approach, resolution of the conflict becomes d ifficu lt and blocking and oscillation occur at the choice point (Hovland and Sears, 1938)* As the instigation to avoid continues to increase in strength, so that the situation comes to simulate the avoidanc e-avoidance type, blocking be­ comes a more frequent type of response, and resolution is more d ifficu lt. (Hovland and Sears, 1938, Klebanoff, 1939* Wearac^ntt Hovland, 1941, and Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

See more

The list of books you might like

Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.