Iowa State University Capstones, Theses and Retrospective Theses and Dissertations Dissertations 2004 Attitudes of native and nonnative speakers of English toward various regional and social U.S. English accents Stacey R. Dent Iowa State University Follow this and additional works at:https://lib.dr.iastate.edu/rtd Part of theBilingual, Multilingual, and Multicultural Education Commons,English Language and Literature Commons, and theFirst and Second Language Acquisition Commons Recommended Citation Dent, Stacey R., "Attitudes of native and nonnative speakers of English toward various regional and social U.S. English accents" (2004). Retrospective Theses and Dissertations. 14451. https://lib.dr.iastate.edu/rtd/14451 This Thesis is brought to you for free and open access by the Iowa State University Capstones, Theses and Dissertations at Iowa State University Digital Repository. It has been accepted for inclusion in Retrospective Theses and Dissertations by an authorized administrator of Iowa State University Digital Repository. For more information, please [email protected]. Attitudes ofnative and nonnative speakersof English toward various regional and social U.S. English accents by StaceyR. Dent A thesis submitted to the graduatefacuhy in partial fulfillment ofthe requirements for the degree of MASTER OFARTS Major: Teaching English as aSecondLanguage/AppliedLinguistics ProgramofStudyCommittee: John Levis, MajorProfessor Roberta Vann StephanieMadon Iowa State University Ames, Iowa 2004 Copyright© StaceyDent, 2004. All rights reserved. Graduate College Iowa State University This is to certifythat the master's thesis of StaceyR. Dent has met the thesis requirements ofIowa State University Signatureshavebeenredacted for privacy Ill TABLEOF CONTENTS LIST OFTABLES iv ABSTRACT v CHAPTER 1: INTRODUCTION 1 Standard Language Ideology 2 Purpose 5 CHAPTER 2: LITERATUREREVIEW 9 Standard American English 9 StandardLanguage Ideology 13 Attitudes ofNativeSpeakers Toward Nonstandard U.S. English Accents 18 Nonnative Speakers' JudgmentsofU.S.EnglishAccents 24 Attitude Formation byNonnativeSpeakers 26 CHAPTER 3: METHODS AND MATERIALS 31 Recording ofthe Stimuli 31 Developmentofthe Tape for Accent Study 34 Raters 35 Procedure 38 Data Analysis 40 CHAPTER4: RESULTS AND DISCUSSION 42 Rating ofSpeech Samples 42 Discussion 57 CHAPTER 5: CONCLUSIONS 66 Applications 66 Recommendations 69 APPENDIX A: READING PASSAGES FORSPEAKERS 71 APPENDIX B; BIOGRAPHICAL INFORMATION QUESTIONNAIRE FOR 73 RATERS APPENDIX C: RATING INSTRUMENT 75 APPENDIX D: SAMPLINGFREQUENCYDISTRUBITIONS OFTHEDATA 77 REFERENCES 80 IV LIST OFTABLES TABLE 3.1.Biographical InformationforNSRaters (N=20) 36 TABLE3.2.BiographicalInformationforNNS6mos Raters(N=34) 37 TABLE3.3.Biographical InformationforNNS2yrRaters (N=26) 38 TABLE4.1.Subjects' MeanRatings(from1(low) to7 (high)) 43 TABLE4.2. Midwestern vs. Southern and Midwestern vs. AAVE for the Rater Groups 45 TABLE 4.3. Differences between NS and NNS6mos 47 TABLE4.4. Differences between NS and NNS2yr 48 TABLE 4.5. Differences between NNS6mos and NNS2yr 50 TABEL4.6. NumberofTraits with Significant Differences between Groups ofRaters 51 TABLE 4.7. Accent Identification by NS (in percent terms) 52 TABLE4.8. Accent Identification by NNS6mos (in percent terms) 53 TABLE4.9. Accent Identification by NNS2yr (in percent terms) 53 TABLE4.10. Means andDifferences for how NNS6mos Rated AAVE and Southern 56 when Identified Correctly and Incorrectly ABSTRACT There is a consistent stigmaassociated with nonstandardU.S. English accents, and language attitude studies have documented that both standard and nonstandard dialect speakers rate speakers ofnonstandard accents lower than standard-accented speakers on a variety ofpersonality characteristics. Whether nonnative speakers ofEnglish share these negative assessments ofnonstandard accents is not clear. The present study investigates the attitudes ofnative as well as nonnative speakers ofEnglish toward various regional and social U.S. English accents and iflength ofstay in the U.S. has an effect on nonnative speakers' adopting language stereotypes similar tonative speakers. Finally, the study seeks to determine.ifthe subjects can correctly identify the accents and ifidentification has an effecton ratings for nonnative speakers. Three U.S. English accents, Midwestern, Southern, and African American Vernacular English (AAVE)wereevaluated usinga7-pointLikertscaleby threegroups ofraters. The groups were native speakers ofEnglish, nonnativespeakers of English presentin the U.S.for six months or less, andnonnativespeakersofEnglishpresentin the U.S.between twoand six years. After rating the speakers, theraters attempted toidentify the accents ofthe speakers. The results showed that all three groups ofraters evaluated the Midwestern-accented speech highest on allpairs ofcharacteristics. Both groups of nonnative speakers had similar ratingsto native speakersfor Midwesternspeech,onlynonnativespeakersin theU.S.fortwo ormoreyearshad similarratings to nativespeakersfortheSouthernspeech. Bothgroupsof nonnativespeakers rated AAVE-accentedspeechlower than native speakers. Nonnative VI speakers were less successful in identifying Midwestern and Southern-accentedEnglish, but were more successful in identifying AAVE-accented English. Skill at identification had little correlation to attitudes expressed by nonnative speakers. The results indicate that time spent in the U.S. is not afactor in adopting the notionof aprestige variety for nonnative speakers. Theresults also indicate that nonnative speakers become more sensitive to regional accents with extended time in the U.S., but that timeis not afactor in nonnative speakers' developing bias toward social accents. CHAPTER1: INTRODUCTION Inthe United States, evidence has shown that dialects which are considered to be nonstandard are stigmatized, and their speakers are associated withnegative stereotypes. We allknow the cliches well. Many people tendto think ofthose who speak with aSouthern accent as slow and unintelligent (Preston, 1996a). They may also characterizeblack speech as ghetto, slang, and low-class. ANew York accent might be associated with arude or working classperson (Niedzielski&Preston2000). Whilethese aresome ofthenegative stereotypes associated with speakers ofnonstandard accents, there are some positive ones as well. Speakerswith nonstandard accentshavealso,forexample,been labeled asbeing friendlier, andmoretrustworthyandhonest, aswellastopossessmorepersonalitythan standardspeakers (Hewett, 1971;Tucker&Lambert,1969). Since languageis soclosely tiedtoidentity, howonespeaks isoftentheyardstickbywhichheisjudged. People tend to usedialectsand accents as awaytosizeoneanotherup (Alvarez&Kolker, 1987). The instant someoneuttersaword, ahostofstereotyped reactions maytakeplace. Ifthespeaker happens tospeakwithastigmatizedaccent, thereactionwilllikelybenegative. Ofcourse, whetherornotthesejudgmentsaretruedoesnotmatter. Thespeechsimplyactsasacueto groupmembership, andthe stereotypedreactionis formedbased onthebeliefswhichonehas aboutparticulargroups ofpeople(Tajfel &Turner, 1979). Thus, speechisaverypowerful tool bywhichwe arejudged. Interestingly, itis not uncommonevenforspeakerswith astigmatizedaccentor dialecttoregardtheirownspeechas"bad"or"improper"(Alvarez&Kolker,1987; Niedzielski &Preston, 2000). Theyoften recognize that iftheywish toleave theregion orto "climbtheladderofsuccess,"theywillhavetolearntospeak"properly** sothattheywillnot bejudgednegativelyorsothattheirprofessional andintellectual abilitieswillnotbe questioned. Thereisoftenthisdistinctionbetweenahome dialect andaprofessionaldialect amongnonstandardspeakers(Alvarez &Kolker, 1987). Conversely, however, other speakerswithnonstandardaccentsrefusetoadoptwhatisconsidered astandardaccent (Wardhaugh, 2002). Tomany,maintainingtheirhomeaccentandvarietyin spiteofthe disadvantagesitmay bring tothem isawayforspeakersofnonstandard dialectstoshow solidaritywith other membersintheirspeechcommunity,topreserve their identitiesandto resist the standardizationoftheir language perhaps due to disappointment in the standard- speakingsociety. Thus,keeping theirhomevarietyhascertain advantageswithin their commimitiesandtheir languagehaswhat iscalledcovertprestige. Thismeansthat although itsprestige is negative, itis "notwithoutitscomforts"(Wardhaugh,2002,p. 349). Standard Language Ideology Such attitudes byboth speakers ofstigmatizeddialects as well as those outside the particularspeech communitycan onlybe understood because their existenceisbased on a comparison to anon-stigmatized variety. Clearly, ifcertaindialects or accents are seen as particularlystigmatized, it must be because thesethey arebeing measured against adialect or accent that is assumed to be superior. Stigmatized dialects and accents are often faulted because they donot sound like what people claim is standard English. While those who make such claims can not explain why, they oftenstate that these varieties are accented and that those who don't speakthe standard arejustnot as intelligentas those who do (Lippi- Green, 1994;Niedzielski & Preston, 2000; Preston 1996a). There is often a senseof*Vhy can't theyjust speakright?* AlthoughtheconceptofstandardEnglishiselusive,thefactthat it isoftenused asamirrorbywhich"bad" Englishcanbemeasured representsthe psychological awarenessthatpeoplehaveoftheexistenceofStandardAmericanEnglish (SAE). Onemight expectsuchpervasivebeliefsaboutwhat is"good" and"bad" Englishto be the resultofsome government agencyregulating standards ofEnglishin the U.S. Howeversuch an agencydoes not exist in this country, as it does in France where the Academie Fran9aiseoversees allregulation ofthe standards ofthe Frenchlanguage. In fact, linguists have had much difficulty defining what standard English is in the United States. It is considereda written as opposed to oralvariety (Wiley and Lukes, 2000). Others claim that it is possible to speak"correct"Englishwith avariety ofaccents, that each region supports its own standard (Falk, 1978). However one attempts to define it, awareness ofastandard indicates thepsychological realityofSAEand can be explainedin terms ofstandard language ideology. Standard language ideologyis defined as, "abias toward an abstracted, idealized, homogenous spoken language which is imposed from above...whichtakes as its model the written language"and which has as its goalthe "suppressionofvariation,"(Lippi- Green, 1994, p. 166). Under such ideology, one variety attains its status as the prestige variety becauseit is the varietyofadominant groupwho is able to impose its variety as superioroverothers (Wiley& Lukes, 2000). The dominance is imposed through a varietyof national institutions, including the educational system, the media, the corporate sector, and the courts (Lippi-Green, 1994). Sincethe dominantvarietyis imposed through these institutions, speakers ofnonstandard regional or social dialects and accents are at a considerablesocial and educational disadvantage. Thus, commandofthe standard varietyis
Description: