ebook img

Association Between Maternal Depressogenic Cognitive Style During Pregnancy and Offspring Cognitive Style 18 Years Later. PDF

0.55 MB·English
Save to my drive
Quick download
Download
Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.

Preview Association Between Maternal Depressogenic Cognitive Style During Pregnancy and Offspring Cognitive Style 18 Years Later.

Article Association Between Maternal Depressogenic Cognitive Style During Pregnancy and Offspring Cognitive Style 18 Years Later Rebecca M. Pearson, Ph.D. Objective: Understanding the origins of offspring’s depressogenic cognitive style negative cognitive style could provide atage18. Charles Fernyhough, Ph.D a means to prevent adult depression. Results: A positive association was ob- Cognitive style is an important target for served between maternal and offspring intervention because although it is not Richard Bentall, Ph.D. cognitive styles: a one-standard-deviation possible to remove the stress and adver- increasein maternal depressogenic cogni- sitiesinpeople’slives,itmaybepossibleto Jonathan Evans, M.D. tive style score during pregnancy was modify interpretation of such adversities significantly associated with a mean in- through cognitive style. Children may de- crease of 0.1 standard deviations in off- Jon Heron, Ph.D. velop a negative cognitive style through springdepressogeniccognitivestylescoreat modelingthestyleoftheirmothers.How- age18.Thiseffectremainedafteradjusting Carol Joinson, Ph.D. ever,findingshavebeeninconsistentonthe formaternalandoffspringdepressionand association.Theauthorstestedthehypoth- explained21%oftheassociationbetween Alan L. Stein, F.R.C.Psych. esis that there is an independent associa- maternalandoffspringdepression. tion between maternal and offspring Conclusions: Although the mechanisms Glyn Lewis, Ph.D. depressogeniccognitivestyle. remaintobeelucidated,thefindingsare Method: Datafromover4,000mothers consistent with the idea that a mother’s andchildrenfromtheAvonLongitudinal cognitivestyle(irrespectiveofherdepres- Study of Parents and Children cohort sion status) influences that of her child. study in the United Kingdom were used Thissuggeststhatinterventionstoimprove to investigate the association between amother’scognitivestylecouldhelppre- maternal depressogenic cognitive style vent her offspring from developing de- before the offspring’s birth and the pressionduringadulthood. (AmJPsychiatry2013;170:434–441) D epression during adulthood is one of the leading article,however,wefocusondepressogeniccognitivestyle causesoftheglobalburdenofdisease(1),makingresearch defined as the tendency to attribute negative events to into prevention a priority. While it is rarely possible to causes that are internal (caused by self), global (have an preventtheoccurrenceoftheadverseeventsandcircum- impact on a wide domain of activities), and stable (are stancesthatarethoughttoincreasetheriskofdepression,it difficulttochange).Cognitivestyleisimportantbecauseit maybepossibletopreventdevelopmentofdepressogenic represents the application of cognitive biases to the self interpretationsofadverseevents,whichmayinturnreduce andtheinterpretationofadverseevents.Indeed,cognitive theriskofdevelopingdepression.Understandingtheorigins style is one of the therapeutic targets of cognitive of depressogenic cognitive style is therefore important in behavioraltherapy,andpreviousresearchhasshownthat strategiestopreventdepression. this style is associated with both concurrent and future Beck’s cognitive theory of depression (2) suggests that episodesofdepression(5). thepresenceofnegativebeliefsystemsregardingtheself, Twinstudiessuggestthattheremaybebothgeneticand the world, and the future underlie the development of environmentalinfluencesondepressogeniccognitivestyle depression. Beck’s theory is consistent with research on (6).However,thereportedgeneticcorrelationsforcogni- attributionalstyles,whichsuggeststhattheapplicationof tivestylearesmall,andsmallerthanthosefordepression such depressogenic beliefs to one’s interpretation of or anxiety disorders themselves (7). Therefore, cognitive negative life events is associated with depression (3). stylemaydeveloplargelythroughexperience.Oneprocess RecentresearchhasextendedBeck’stheorytomorebasic bywhichnegativecognitivestylemaydevelopisthrough cognitive biases. For example, attentional bias toward theexplanationsofeventsprovidedbycaregivers,partic- negative stimuli is also associated with vulnerability to ularly mothers. Several studies have suggested that depression(4).Suchbiasesmayrepresentalowercogni- negativeandcriticalmaternalfeedbackisassociatedwith tive level of depressogenic beliefs: the beliefs we hold depressogenic offspring cognitive styles (8–10). Mothers’ direct the information we bias and vice versa. In this owncognitivestylemayalsoprovideamodelforthechild 434 ajp.psychiatryonline.org AmJPsychiatry170:4,April2013 PEARSON,FERNYHOUGH,BENTALL,ETAL. toimitate(11,12);basedonobservationsoftheirmother’s in southwest England who had an estimated date of delivery inferencesaboutherselfandcircumstances,childrenwill between April 1, 1991, and December 31, 1992, were invited to takepart.Thechildrenof15,247pregnancieswererecruited(20). developtheirowncognitivestyle. The representative nature of the original ALSPAC sample has However, there are also theoretical reasons for ques- beeninvestigatedbycomparisonwiththe1991NationalCensus tioning whether there is any direct association between dataofresidentsinthecountyofAvon(20).Ethicalapprovalfor maternalandoffspringcognitivestyles.First,itispossible the study was obtained from the ALSPAC Law and Ethics that a mother’s negative beliefs about herself are not Committeeand theLocalResearchEthicsCommittees.Written sufficiently expressed in her behavior to be modeled. consent was obtained from all participants after they received a description of the study. Detailed information has been Second,itispossiblethatthechildmayusethemother’s collected on the cohort since early pregnancy (20; see also attributions to reach diametrically opposite conclusions. http://www.alspac.bris.ac.uk). In this study, we used data from Forexample,ifthemothermakesanexternalattribution thesubsampleofALSPACsingletonoffspringwhoattendedthe and blames the child for her misfortunes, the child may researchclinicatage18. learntoblamehim-orherself(8). Measures Althoughsomestudiesreportcorrelationsbetweenma- Offspring cognitive style. The Cognitive Style Questionnaire– ternal and offspring cognitive style (8, 13, 14), at least as Short Form (CSQ-SF), which was developed from the original manystudiesfailtofindevidenceforanyassociation(10,15, Cognitive Style Questionnaire (21) and has been shown to be 16). This inconsistency may reflect a failure to detect real reliableandvalidforuseinadolescents(22),wasadministeredto associations because the studies were limited to cross- theALSPACchildrenatthefourthTeenFocusClinic(meanage: sectionaldesigns(14,16)orhadsmallsamplesizes(,400) 17 years, 10 months). The CSQ-SF presents eight hypothetical (8–10, 13). Previous studies have also measured mother’s events relating to failures in academic, employment, and in- terpersonal relationships. For each event, participants are in- cognitivestyleaftertheindexchildwasborn,thusfailingto structed to vividly imagine themselves in that situation and eliminatethepossibilityofreversecausality. thinkcarefullyaboutthereasonfortheevent.Next,usingaLikert In addition, previous studies have limited their investi- scale of agreement from 1–5, participants rate the extent to gationstowithinchildhoodandearlyadolescence(9,10,13). whichthisreasonwascausedbyinternalversusexternalfactors (themselves or others), specific versus global factors (has an Itis,however,importanttounderstandtheantecedentsof impactonallareasorthisspecificsituation),andstableversus cognitivestylesthatarecarriedintoadulthood,wherethe unstable factors (will persist or not) and the extent to which it main public health burdens of depression are manifest. reflects theirself-worth(meansthey are flawed)(Figure 1). For Cognitivestylesmayalsobeopentochanginginfluences eachscenario,twoitemsrelatetoeachofthefourdimensions, (10), and as the child develops, the mother may become resultingin64items,withtotalscoresrangingfrom64to320.A lessinfluentialincomparisontopeers(17). fifth dimension relating to negative consequences was omitted from the CSQ-SF. Higher scores indicate a more negative style. Finally, it is unclear whether the association between Total scores showed a normal distribution, with a Cronbach’s maternal and offspring cognitive styles is independent of alpha(measuringinternalconsistency)of0.88,whichiscompa- maternaldepression.Maternalcognitivestyleandmaternal rabletopreviousstudies(22).Aprincipal-componentsanalysisof depression are strongly related (18), and maternal de- thescoresforthefourdimensionsindicatedthatasinglefactor with an eigenvalue of 3.25 explained 65% of the variance. All pression is also a risk factor for offspring depressogenic subscalescoresloadedonthisfactor(loadingsrangedfrom0.37 cognitivestyle(10).Partofthisassociationmaybeexplained to0.85),whichiscomparabletopreviousstudies(22).Weused bythecognitivestyleofdepressedmothers.However,the CSQ-SFtotalscoreastheprimaryoutcomevariable. effect of maternal depression could also operate through Atotalof4,693adolescentscompletedatleastoneitemonthe mechanisms that are unrelated to cognitive style. For questionnaire, and 3,387 completed all 64 items. For those who completedatleast75%oftheitems(equivalenttoatleastsixofthe example,maternaldepressionmayactasadversityforthe eightscenarios),missingitemswereimputedfromtheindividuals’ child or have an impact on the child’s cognitive style medianscoresacrossallotheritemsinthesubscalefromwhich throughitsassociationwithlesssensitiveparenting(19). itemsweremissing.Resultsarereportedfortheimputedscores. In this study, we sought to improve on previous in- Allanalyseswererepeatedwithcompleteitemscoresonly,andthe vestigations by using data from over 4,000 families from resultswerecomparable(dataavailableonrequest). the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children Maternal cognitive style during pregnancy. A broader mea- (ALSPAC)cohortintheUnitedKingdomtoinvestigatethe sure of intrapersonal sensitivity (23) given to mothers as part association between maternal cognitive style measured ofaquestionnairemailedoutat18weeksofpregnancywasused to derive a measure of maternal cognitive style (18). Six items during pregnancy and offspring cognitive style 18 years relatingtonegativecognitionsoutlinedinBeck’scognitivetheory later and how any such association was related to the ofdepressionwereselectedonatheoreticalbasisinaprevious associationbetweenmaternalandoffspringdepression. study (18). These items originated from the Dysfunctional AttitudesScale,whichhasbeenvalidatedagainstareviewer-led Method measureofcognitivedistortionsusinghypotheticalstories(24). Selected items were judged to measure negative cognitions according toBeck’s theory;two authors(G.L.and J.E.) selected Participants theseitemsindependently.Itemswereexcludediftheyincluded The sample comprised participants from ALSPAC. All preg- wordsthatwerelikelytobeconfoundedbycurrentmood,such nant women residing in the former Avon Health Authority as “worry” or “feel.” The internal consistency of the items was 435 AmJPsychiatry170:4,April2013 ajp.psychiatryonline.org MATERNALANDOFFSPRINGCOGNITIVESTYLES FIGURE1.OffspringandMaternalCognitiveStyleItems Offspring Cognitive Style Items Relation to Depressogenic Thinking Example Scenario: Imagine you go to a party and people are not interested in you. Think carefully about the reason for people not being interested in you. The reason is my fault Internal The reason affects all areas of my life Global The reason will cause the same negative event in the future Stable The reason means there is something wrong with me as a person Self-worth Maternal Cognitive Style Items Relation to Depressogenic Thinking I avoid saying what I think for fear of being rejected Global and self-worth If others knew the real me, they would not like me Internal and self-worth If other people knew what I am really like, they would think less of me Internal and self-worth I always expect criticism Global, stable, and self-worth I don’t like people to really know me Global, stable, and internal My value as a person depends enormously on what others think of me Global and self-worth high(Cronbach’salpha=0.77).Eachitemwasratedforagreement depressive ideas, poor concentration, sleep problems, and fa- ona4-pointLikertscale,withpossiblescoresrangingfrom0to3. tiguetoaccountforsubthresholdsymptoms. Summed scores could range from 0 to 18, with higher scores reflecting more negative cognitions. A total of 12,175 mothers Statistical Analysis completed the six items, with a mean score of 4.6 (SD=3.4). Wefirstexaminedtherelationshipbetweenmaternalcognitive Higher scores for mothers who were not currently depressed styleandoffspringcognitivestyleinaseriesoflinearregression werepredictiveoffutureepisodesofdepressionyearsafterbirth models. The exposurevariable was maternalnegative cognitive (18).Thenegativecognitionsendorsedbythemothersrelateto style, treated as a continuous score. The outcome variable was the dimensions on the CSQ-SF. For example, to agree with the offspringtotal CSQ-SF score.We examined aunivariate model, statement “I always expect criticism” reflects low self-worth, thenintroducedpotential confoundingvariables(maternaland which, as indicated by the endorsement of “always,” is both offspring depression) separately into the model to investigate aglobalandastableattribution.Asthismeasurewastakenasan their impact on the main association. Although later maternal index of maternal cognitive style, it is referred to as such depression measures were available, they were not included in hereafter(seeFigure1). ourmodelsbecauseofthestrongcorrelationamongtimingsof maternaldepression.However,werepeatedallanalysesreported Confounding variables. Variables previously shown to be as- here with antenatal replaced by postnatal timing of depression sociatedwithmaternalandoffspringcognitivestyleswerecon- and obtained comparable results (data available on request). trolled for (10). Maternal characteristics were derived from Therewerenolatermeasuresofmaternalcognitivestyle. questionnaires given during pregnancy. These included age, We examined a final multivariate model that included all education level, social class, (ranked from 1 to 5, with lower confoundingvariables,andthenwerepeatedthefinalmultivar- scores indicating higher status), parity, smoking during preg- iate analysis excluding mothers who exceeded thresholds for nancy,anddepressionmeasuredusingtheEdinburghPostnatal depression at the time they completed the cognitive style Depression Scale (25) at 18 weeks of pregnancy on the same measure. Analyses were conducted using Stata, version 12 questionnaire from which the measure of maternal cognitive (StataCorp,CollegeStation,Tex.). style was derived. The Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale Mediation analyses. To investigate whether the association is a 10-item self-report questionnaire specifically designed to between maternal and offspring cognitive style mediated any screenforperinataldepression;scores.12havebeenshownto association between maternal and offspring depression, we have a high sensitivity (81.1%) and specificity (95.7%) in investigated the association between maternal and offspring predicting major depressive disorder (26). We also included depression before and after including cognitive styles and offspringgenderandoffspringdepression,whichwasmeasured performedamediationanalysisusingMplus. with the Clinical Interview Schedule–Revised (27) at the same researchclinicastheCSQ-SF. Missing data. In a sensitivity analysis, we used multiple im- putations to account for missing data. Given the availability of Offspringdepression.TheClinicalInterviewSchedule–Revised information on sociodemographic variables, we assumed that is a self-administered, computerized interview that establishes missingness is dependent on observed data and used multiple theseverityofsymptomsthatconstituteanxietyanddepression chained equations using all study variables and sociodemo- disorders using algorithms based on ICD-10 criteria (27). The graphic indicators to impute 30 data sets. We then repeated version we used derives an ICD-10 diagnosis as well as symp- analysesacrosstheimputeddatasetscombiningestimatesusing tomseverityscoresfordepression,depressivethoughts,anxiety, Rubin’s rules (28). Given the difficulty in imputing missing panic, phobia, sleep, concentration, and fatigue, on a scale of outcomes (28), we initially imputed only up to the sample for 0–4.Weusedabinaryvariable(depressed,notdepressed);cases which complete cognitive style (CSQ-SF) outcome data were werethosewithaprimarydiagnosisofmild,moderate,orsevere available (N=3,845). However, given the association between depression using ICD-10 criteria. We also used a depressive CSQ-SF and mood, we used five earlier measures of mood to symptom score derived by summing scores for depression, predict CSQ-SF and repeated the imputation model up to 436 ajp.psychiatryonline.org AmJPsychiatry170:4,April2013 PEARSON,FERNYHOUGH,BENTALL,ETAL. TABLE1.CharacteristicsofCompleteCaseSampleandOverallAvonLongitudinalStudyof ParentsandChildren(ALSPAC) Sample CompleteCaseSample Characteristic (N=2,528) ALSPACSamplea p N % TotalN N % Maternaleducation 8,827 ,0.001 Universitydegree 572 23 982 11 Alevel(noncompulsory,afterage16) 771 31 1,957 22 Olevel/vocation(compulsory,beforeage16) 1,036 41 4,345 49 NoneorCertificateofSecondaryEducation 149 6 1,543 17 SocialClass 7,280 ,0.001 1(higherprofessional) 228 9 351 5 2(professional) 968 38 2,116 29 3(skilled) 1,135 45 3,828 53 4(unskilled) 169 7 795 11 5(unemployed) 27 1 187 3 Pregnancyandmaternaldata 10,072 First-bornchild 1,286 51 4,374 43 ,0.001 Femalechild 1,411 56 7,977 46 ,0.001 Mean SD TotalN Mean SD Mother’sage(years) 29.7 4.3 27.6 5.0 ,0.001 Mother’sdepressionscoreduringpregnancy 6.1 4.4 7.2 4.9 ,0.001 Maternalcognitivestyle 5.0 3.7 5.0 3.5 0.758 Childdepressionandcognitivestyledata 1,669 ChildCSQscore 162 20 161 20 0.794 N % N % Childdepressiondiagnosisatage18,based 181 7 148 9 0.044 ontheClinicalInterviewSchedule–Revised aForthecoresingletonALSPACsamplenotinthecompletecasesample,N=11,089.Percentagesarebasedontheproportionofthosewith dataonthesevariables,asindicatedinthe“TotalN”column. a starting sample with at least one measure of mood or tem- TABLE2.MeanScoreontheCognitiveStyleQuestionnaire– perament as well as exposure (N=10,322). This model should Short Form Among Offspring at Age 18, by Maternal be interpreted with caution, however, as our ability to predict CognitiveStyleScoreDuringPregnancya CSQ-SFscoreswaslimited. Score MaternalCognitiveStyleTertile Mean SD 95%CI Results Lowesttertile(N=1,076) 161 20.0 160–162 Atotal of3,845offspringprovideduseabledataonthe Middletertile(N=877) 162 20.0 161–164 CSQ-SFatage18.Fromthissample,3,320oftheirmothers Highesttertile(N=813) 164 20.2 163–165 had provided maternal cognitive style data during preg- a Maternalcognitivestylescoresaregroupedintotertilesrepresent- inglowscores(0–2),moderatescores(3–5),andhighscores(6–18). nancy.Thissamplewasfurtherreducedto2,528mothers The table demonstrates a dose-response relationship between andchildrenforwhomcompletedataforallconfounding maternal cognitive style and offspring cognitive style score, variableswereavailable.Demographiccharacteristics for indicating that mothers with higher cognitive style scores have offspringwithhigherscores. thesamplewithcompletedataascomparedwiththerest ofALSPACaresummarizedinTable1. cognitive style. For a one-standard-deviation increase in AssociationsWith Offspring CognitiveStyle maternal depression score, offspring cognitive style score Meanoffspringcognitivestyle(CSQ-SF)scores,bylevel increased by 1.28 points (95% CI=0.40–2.17; p=0.005). ofmaternalcognitivestyle,arepresentedinTable2.Linear However, the increase fell to 0.17 (95% CI=–0.002 to 0.36; regressionanalyses provided evidencefor a positiveasso- p=0.09)afteradjustmentformaternalcognitivestyle. ciationbetweenmaternalandoffspringcognitivestyle.This association held in the sample for whom complete data Associations WithOffspring Depression and Mediating RoleofCognitive Styles wereavailableforconfoundingvariablesafterweincluded adjustments for maternal and offspring depression and Cognitive style scores for depressed offspring were 14 additional confounding variables, excluding mothers who points (0.8 standard deviations) higher (95% CI=12–18; were depressed at baseline, and imputing missing data p,0.001)thanthosefortheirnondepressedpeers.There (Table 3). There was also evidence for a univariate wasalsoanassociationbetweenmaternalcognitivestyle association between maternal depression and offspring andoffspringdepression(oddsratio=1.18,95%CI=1.01–1.40; 437 AmJPsychiatry170:4,April2013 ajp.psychiatryonline.org MATERNALANDOFFSPRINGCOGNITIVESTYLES TABLE 3.Mean Increase in Cognitive Style Questionnaire–Short Form (CSQ-SF) Score Among Offspring for Each 6-Point IncreaseinMaternalCognitiveStyleScorea IncreaseinOffspringCSQ-SFScore Model b 95%CI p Model1a:UnivariateassociationwithchildCSQ-SFscore,allavailable 2.55 1.38–3.72 ,0.001 data(N=3,320) Model1b:UnivariateassociationwithchildCSQ-SFscore,complete 2.86 1.51–4.22 ,0.001 cases(N=2,528) Model2:Adjustedforconcurrentadolescentdepression 2.59 1.26–3.90 ,0.001 Model3:Adjustedformother’santenataldepression 2.45 0.79–4.10 0.004 Model4:Alladjustmentsb 1.96 0.53–3.40 0.007 Model5:Asinmodel4plusexclusionofthosewhosemotherswho 1.81 0.22–3.40 0.025 weredepressedatbaseline(N=2,209) Model6:Asinmodel4,withimputationsformissingdataforthose 2.03 0.79–3.25 ,0.001 whohadCSQ-SFscores(N=3,845) Model7:Asinmodel4,withimputationsformissingdataandCSQ-SF 2.40 0.90–3.86 0.002 scores(N=10,322) a Models 1 and 2 show results for univariate linear regression analyses; models 2 and 3 show results after adjustments for offspring and maternaldepression;model4showsresultsafteradjustmentforconfoundingvariables;andmodels6and7showresultsaftermissingdata imputation. b indicates the increase in CSQ-SF score for each 6-point increase in maternal cognitive style score, which represents the approximatedifferencebetweenthelowestandhighesttertiles.Models2–4wereconductedforthoseinthesampleforwhomcompletedata wereavailable(N=2,528).Model5wasrestrictedtothoseforwhomcompletedatawereavailableandwhosemothershaddepressionscores belowthresholdatbaseline. bAdjustments were made for adolescent depression continuous score and diagnosis indicator, maternal antenatal depression total score, maternalage,maternaleducation,socialclass,maternalhistoryofdepression,childgender,andparityofmotherwithindexchild. p=0.049),whichwasreducedafteradjustmentforoffspring Strengths andLimitations cognitivestyle(oddsratio=1.10,95%CI=0.93–1.31;p=0.248). Strengthsofthestudyincludethelargesamplesize,the Finally, there was an association between maternal de- long-term follow-up spanning the life of offspring from pression and offspring depression. For a one-standard- before birth into adulthood, and the availability of con- deviationincreaseinmaternaldepressionscore,theodds founding variables, particularly concurrent measures of of offspring depression were increased by 23% (odds bothmaternalandoffspringdepression. ratio=1.23, 95% CI=1.04–1.44; p=0.013). This associa- Alimitationofthestudyisthelosstofollow-upfromthe tion was reduced after maternal and offspring cognitive original sample. Young adults who attended the clinic at styleswere included inthe model (odds ratio=1.15, 95% age 18 were less likely to come from poorer families. CI=0.96–1.39;p=0.138).AsshowninFigure2,mediation However,sensitivityanalysesaccountingformissingdata analysis provides evidence that 21% of the association provided no evidence that missing data introduced bias. between maternal and offspring depression was medi- Another limitation is that the measurement of maternal atedbymaternalandoffspringcognitivestyles. and offspring cognitive style differed. The maternal measure related to negative cognitive style in general, whereas the offspring measure involved attribution of cognitive styles to hypothetical events. According to Discussion cognitive theories, the two measures should reflect the An increase of approximately one standard deviation same underlying constructs. For example, if a mother in maternal cognitive style score during pregnancy was alwaysexpectscriticism,itseemsreasonabletoinferthat associatedwithanincreaseofapproximately0.1standard this outlook would be applied to the attributions of deviationsinoffspringcognitivestylescoreatage18.The negative events. Nonetheless, there was likely to have small effect size may explain previous inconsistent find- beenconsiderablemeasurementerrorinmeasuringcog- ings because it suggests that this association may have nitivestyles,forthematernalmeasureinparticular;some beenmissedinsmallerstudieslackingsufficientstatistic- subjects with negative cognitive styles may not have al power. Our analyses provide evidence that maternal reportedthisinthefewquestionsavailable,inwhichcase andoffspringcognitivestylesexplained21%oftheassocia- their score on the measure would not accurately reflect tionbetweenmaternalandoffspringdepression.Itisstrik- theircognitivestyle.Thiswillhaveaddednoisetothedata, ing that the association between maternal and offspring likelyleadingtoanunderestimateofanyassociation. cognitive styles should remain despite the possible re- Therearealsoseveralquestionsthatourfindingscannot duction in maternal influence as the child ages and the answer. For example, the stability of maternal cognitive potential for the mother’s cognitive style to change stylethroughoutthechild’slifewasunclear,whichlimits throughthechild’slife. our understanding as to when it influences the child. 438 ajp.psychiatryonline.org AmJPsychiatry170:4,April2013 PEARSON,FERNYHOUGH,BENTALL,ETAL. FIGURE 2.Theoretical Pathways Linking Maternal Depression and Maternal Cognitive Style With Offspring Cognitive Style andDepressiona (Unadjusted association between maternal and offspring depression, 0.088, p=0.014) Offspring depression Maternal depression 0.070 diagnosis at age 18 in pregnancy (total score) (p=0.048) (yes/no) 0.450 0.024 0.332 (p<0.001) (p=0.317) (p<0.001) Offspring cognitive Maternal cognitive style 0.073 style at age 18 in pregnancy (total score) (p=0.002) (total score) aAnalysisbasedonparticipantsforwhomcompletedatawereavailable(N=2,528).Inthediagram,thearrowsrepresentregressionsandthus associations between variables. All regression path coefficients are standardized, so all effects sizes are directly comparable. The total association between maternal depression and offspring depression at age 18 was 0.088 (95% CI=0.017–0.159, p=0.014). This total effect comprisesalldirectandindirectpathwaysfrommaternaltooffspringdepression.Thiswouldcomprisethedirectassociationbeforecognitive stylesareconsideredinthemodel.Incontrast,oncematernalandchildcognitivestylesareconsidered,thedirectassociationdropsto0.070 (p=0.048).Thetotalindirecteffectthroughmotherandoffspringcognitivestyleswas0.019(95%CI=0.017–0.020,p=0.010),ascalculatedin Mplusfromtheproductoftheindirectpathwaysinthefigure.Thus,21%(0.019/0.088)oftheoriginaltotalassociationbetweenmaternaland childdepressionwasmediatedbymotherandchildcognitivestyles.Themodelfitindicesweregood;x2=0.140,df=1,p=0.708,rootmean squareerrorofapproximation=0.0,95%CI=0.00–0.038,comparativefitindex=1.0;standarderrorsandthusconfidenceintervalsofdirect andindirecteffectswereestimatedusingbootstrapping.Theestimatorwasweightedleastsquares. Furthermore, the lack of a parenting measure limits our is initially brought about, the important point is that understandingastohowmaternalcognitivestyleinfluences maternal depression and cognitive style were strongly thechild.Finally,althoughpreviousstudieshavereported correlated in pregnancy and that the association with limitedheritabilityofdepressogeniccognitivestyles(6,29), cognitivestylespartiallyexplainedtheeffectsofmaternal unmeasuredsharedgeneticfactorsmayalsoexplainpartof depression. Transmission of cognitive styles is therefore theassociation. one potential pathway from maternal to offspring de- pressionthatismodifiableandwouldprovideatargetfor Mechanisms prevention. Furthermore, maternal cognitive style was One explanation for the association between mother associated with offspring cognitive style irrespective of and offspring cognitive styles is that negative cognitive maternal depression. Thus, maternal cognitive style is styleis amarker for depression andthereforetheresults a potential target for prevention of offspring depression reflecttheestablishedassociationbetweenmaternaland evenifthemotherisnotdepressedatthatpoint. offspringdepression(12,30).However,theassociationre- Interventions mainedafteradjustments for maternal andoffspringde- pression,indicatingthatthisisnotalikelyexplanation.In There are two possible strategies that might prevent contrast, maternal and child cognitive styles mediated the transmission of cognitive styles. First, maternal asignificantpartoftheassociationbetweenmaternaland cognitivestylecouldbemodified.Forexample,cognitive offspring depression. Because maternal depression and therapy is designed to modify cognitive styles, and maternal cognitive style were measured on the same evidencesuggeststhatithasalastingpositiveinfluence occasion,wecannotdeterminethedirectionofcausality. on cognitive style (31). As described above, it is also Previous evidence suggests that cognitive style leads to possible that maternal depression leads to negative depression(18);however,depressedstatescouldalsolead cognitive styles, and thus in depressed mothers anti- tonegativecognitivestyle.Whicheverwaytheassociation depressant treatment could prevent development of a 439 AmJPsychiatry170:4,April2013 ajp.psychiatryonline.org MATERNALANDOFFSPRINGCOGNITIVESTYLES PatientPerspectives (cid:129) A 19-year-old female patient presented to psychiatric (cid:129) The following exchange occurred in a face-to-face serviceswithdepression.Shebegantreatmentwithcognitive- interactionbetweenamotherandheryounginfant.The behavioral therapy. During discussions of her conditional infant appeared to be sleepy, and the mother initially beliefswithhertherapist,thepatientdescribedmemoriesof commentedonthisfact.However,afewmomentslater, her mother’s reactions and interpretations of events in her withoutanychangeintheinfant’sbehavior,themother andhersister’slives:“Ifwecamesecondinclass,mumwould said,“Ithinkyou’reangryatme.”Aplausibleexplanation say, “It’s a shame you never come first in anything.” If my isthatthemotherwasinappropriatelyattributingtoher sister or I made any mistakes, mum would get upset and childnegativeandcriticalemotionsexpectedfromothers. thinkshehadfailedasaparent.Shewouldsaythatwewere Ifachildgrowsupwithfrequentexposuretonegativeand uselessandwouldneverbesuccessful.Ibegantothinkthat critical parental evaluations, these will arguably contrib- whateverIdid,inwhateverareaofmylife,Iwouldneverbe utetotheformationofanegativemodelofself.(Example goodenough,everythingIdidjustcausedupset.”(Example provided by J. Cassidy, personal communication, July drawnfromG.L.’sclinicalexperience.) 2012.) negativecognitivestyle.Second,ratherthanfocusingon the findings suggest that modifying maternal cognitive changing the mother’s cognitive style, interventions style irrespective of the current presence of maternal could focus on preventing transmission by changing depression could help prevent the development of whatamothersaysandhowshebehaves.Topreventthe a depressogenic cognitive style in offspring. Given the imitationofcognitivestyles,interventionscouldworkon association between cognitive style and clinical levels of preventingmothersfromprovidingexamplesofnegative depression in this and other studies (35), these findings cognitivestyle.Forexample,commentingthatthechild’s provideanotherpossibleroutetopreventingdepression. act was bad rather than that the child is bad would be modifying a global and personal attribution that is specific and external. Negative maternal cognitive style Received May 23, 2012; revisions received Aug. 1 and Aug. 22, 2012; accepted Aug. 30, 2012 (doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2012. may also result in less specific forms of negative par- 12050673).FromtheAcademicUnitofPsychiatry,OakfieldHouse, enting,whichmayalsoneedtobetargeted(32).Forex- OakfieldGrove,UniversityofBristol,Bristol,U.K.;theDepartmentof Psychology,DurhamUniversity,Durham,U.K.;theInstituteofMental ample, mother-child interventions such as video and Behavioural Sciences, University of Liverpool, Liverpool, U.K.; feedback have been shown to modify mothers’ expres- and the Department of Psychiatry, Oxford University, Oxford, U.K. sion of negative language and behavior and increase AddresscorrespondencetoDr.Pearson([email protected]. uk). responsiveness (33). Finally, a mother may apply her Dr. Bentall has received a speaking honorarium from Lilly. The negative beliefs to her interpretation of child behavior: other authors report no financial relationships with commercial amotherwhoalwaysfeelscriticizedmayperceivethat— interests. and act as though—her child is critical of her (see the The UK Medical Research Council (Grant 74882), the Wellcome Trust (Grant 076467), and the University of Bristol provide core second patient perspective). Children who are told that support for the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children they hold negative and critical thoughts may come (ALSPAC).ThisstudywassupportedbyaWellcomegrantheldbyDr. Lewis. to believe that they are negative people. In this case, Theauthorsaregratefultothefamilieswhotookpartinthestudy, interventions could work on improving the mother’s the midwives for help in recruiting them, and the whole ALSPAC awareness of the child’s mental processes, or mind- team,whichincludesinterviewers,computerandlaboratorytechni- mindedness(34).Forexample,changingamother’sfocus cians, clerical workers, research scientists, volunteers, managers, receptionists, and nurses. Patient Perspectives were provided in toherchild’sratherthanherownmentalstatesmayhelp collaborationwithJ.CassidyandE.Meins. heractappropriatelytothechild’sneeds. Conclusions References To our knowledge, this is the first study to provide 1. WorldHealthOrganization:TheGlobalBurdenofDisease:2004 evidenceforaninfluence,persistingintoearlyadulthood, Update.Geneva,WorldHealthOrganization,2008 2. Beck AT: Cognition, affect, and psychopathology. Arch Gen of a mother’s cognitive style on her offspring’s cognitive Psychiatry1971;24:495–500 style.Theeffectsizeisrelativelysmall,butgiventhedose- 3. AlloyLB,AbramsonLY,MetalskyGI,HartlageS:Thehopeless- response relationship, the 18-year longitudinal time ness theory of depression: attributional aspects. Br J Clin Psy- chol1988;27:5–21 frame, the low-risk sample, and potential measurement 4. Joiner TE Jr, Wagner KD: Parental, child-centered attributions error resulting in underestimation of any associations, and outcome: a meta-analytic review with conceptual and suchaneffectislikelytobemeaningful.Themechanisms methodologicalimplications.JAbnormChildPsychol1996;24: underlying this effect remain to be established, although 37–52 440 ajp.psychiatryonline.org AmJPsychiatry170:4,April2013 PEARSON,FERNYHOUGH,BENTALL,ETAL. 5. PrinsteinMJ,AikinsJW:Cognitivemoderatorsofthelongitudi- 22. MeinsE,McCarthy-JonesS,FernyhoughC,LewisG,BentallRP, nal association between peer rejection and adolescent de- Alloy LB: Assessing negative cognitive style: development and pressivesymptoms.JAbnormChildPsychol2004;32:147–158 validationofashort-formversionoftheCognitiveStyleQues- 6. LauJY,RijsdijkF,EleyTC:Ithink,thereforeIam:atwinstudyof tionnaire.PersIndividDif2012;52:581–585 attributional style in adolescents. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 23. Boyce P, Parker G: Development of a scale to measure in- 2006;47:696–703 terpersonalsensitivity.AustNZJPsychiatry1989;23:341–351 7. ZavosHM,RijsdijkFV,GregoryAM,EleyTC:Geneticinfluences 24. Weissman AN, Beck AT: Development and validation of the onthecognitivebiasesassociatedwithanxietyanddepression DysfunctionalAttitudesScale:apreliminaryinvestigation.Pro- symptomsinadolescents.JAffectDisord2010;124:45–53 ceedingsofthe62ndAnnualMeetingoftheAmericanEduca- 8. AlloyLB,AbramsonLY,TashmanNA,BerrebbiDS,HoganME, tionalResearchAssociation.Toronto,March1978 WhitehouseWG,CrossfieldAG,MoroccoA:Developmentalori- 25. Cox JL, Holden JM, Sagovsky R: Detection of postnatal de- ginsofcognitive vulnerability todepression:parenting,cogni- pression: development of the 10-item Edinburgh Postnatal tive,andinferentialfeedbackstylesoftheparentsofindividuals DepressionScale.BrJPsychiatry1987;150:782–786 athighandlowcognitiveriskfordepression.CognitTherRes 26. MurrayL,CarothersAD:ThevalidationoftheEdinburghPost- 2001;25:397–423 natalDepressionScaleonacommunitysample.BrJPsychiatry 9. GarberJ,FlynnC:PredictorsofDepressiveCognitionsinYoung 1990;157:288–290 Adolescents.CognitTherRes2001;25:353–376 27. LewisG,PelosiAJ,ArayaR,DunnG:Measuringpsychiatricdis- 10. MezulisA,FunasakiK,HydeJS:Negativecognitivestyletrajec- orderinthecommunity:astandardizedassessmentforuseby tories in the transition to adolescence. J Clin Child Adolesc layinterviewers.PsycholMed1992;22:465–486 Psychol2011;40:318–331 28. White IR, Royston P, Wood AM: Multiple imputation using 11. BanduraA,HustonAC:Identificationasaprocessofincidental chainedequations:issuesandguidanceforpractice.StatMed learning.JAbnormSocPsychol1961;63:311–318 2011;30:377–399 12. Goodman SH, Gotlib IH: Risk for psychopathology in the chil- 29. FarmerA,HarrisT,RedmanK,MahmoodA,SadlerS,McGuffin dren of depressed mothers: a developmental model for un- P: The Cardiff Depression Study: a sib-pair study of dysfunc- derstanding mechanisms of transmission. Psychol Rev 1999; tional attitudes in depressed probands and healthy control 106:458–490 subjects.PsycholMed2001;31:627–633 13. SeligmanME,PetersonC,KaslowNJ,TanenbaumRL,AlloyLB, 30. Goodman SH, Rouse MH, Connell AM, Broth MR, Hall CM, Abramson LY: Attributional style and depressive symptoms Heyward D: Maternal depression and child psychopathology: amongchildren.JAbnormPsychol1984;93:235–238 a meta-analytic review. Clin Child Fam Psychol Rev 2011; 14: 14. StarkKD,SchmidtKL,JoinerTEJr:Cognitivetriad:relationship 1–27 todepressivesymptoms,parents’cognitivetriad,andperceived 31. JarrettRB,Vittengl JR,DoyleK,ClarkLA:Changesincognitive parentalmessages.JAbnormChildPsychol1996;24:615–631 content during and following cognitive therapy for recurrent 15. OliverJM,BergerLS:Depression,parent-offspringrelationships, depression: substantial and enduring, but not predictive of andcognitivevulnerability.JSocBehavPers1992;7:415–429 changeindepressivesymptoms.JConsultClinPsychol2007;75: 16. Kaslow NJ,Rehm LP,Pollack SL,SiegelAW: Attributional style 432–446 andself-controlbehaviorindepressedandnondepressedchil- 32. Stein A, Craske MG,Lehtonen A, Harvey A,Savage-McGlynn E, dren and their parents. J Abnorm Child Psychol 1988; 16: Davies B, Goodwin J, Murray L, Cortina-Borja M, Counsell N: 163–175 Maternalcognitionsandmother-infantinteractioninpostnatal 17. Stevens EA, Prinstein MJ: Peer contagion of depressogenic at- depressionandgeneralizedanxietydisorder.JAbnormPsychol tributional styles among adolescents: a longitudinal study. J (Epubaheadofprint,Jan30,2012) AbnormChildPsychol2005;33:25–37 33. Stein A, Woolley H, Senior R, Hertzmann L, Lovel M, Lee J, 18. EvansJ,HeronJ,LewisG,ArayaR,WolkeD;ALSPACstudyteam: CooperS,WheatcroftR,ChallacombeF,PatelP,Nicol-Harper Negativeself-schemasandtheonsetofdepressioninwomen: R, Menzes P, Schmidt A, Juszczak E, Fairburn CG: Treating longitudinalstudy.BrJPsychiatry2005;186:302–307 disturbances in the relationship between mothers with bu- 19. Lovejoy MC, Graczyk PA, O’Hare E, Neuman G: Maternal de- limic eating disorders and their infants: a randomized, con- pression and parenting behavior: ameta-analytic review. Clin trolled trial of video feedback. Am J Psychiatry 2006; 163: PsycholRev2000;20:561–592 899–906 20. BoydA,GoldingJ,MacleodJ,LawlorDA,FraserA,HendersonJ, 34. Meins E, Fernyhough C, Fradley E, Tuckey M: Rethinking ma- Molloy L, Ness A, Ring S, Davey Smith G: Cohort Profile: the ternal sensitivity: mothers’ comments on infants’ mental pro- “Childrenofthe90s”:theindexoffspringoftheAvonLongitu- cesses predict security of attachment at 12 months. J Child dinal Study of Parents and Children. Int J Epidemiol (Epub PsycholPsychiatry2001;42:637–648 aheadofprint,Apr16,2012) 35. MezulisAH,AbramsonLY,HydeJS,HankinBL:Isthereauni- 21. HaeffelGJ,GibbBE,MetalskyGI,AlloyLB,AbramsonLY,Hankin versalpositivitybiasinattributions?Ameta-analyticreviewof BL,JoinerTEJr,SwendsenJD:Measuringcognitivevulnerability individual, developmental, and cultural differences in the to depression: development and validation of the Cognitive self-serving attributional bias. Psychol Bull 2004; 130: StyleQuestionnaire.ClinPsycholRev2008;28:824–836 711–747 441 AmJPsychiatry170:4,April2013 ajp.psychiatryonline.org

See more

The list of books you might like

Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.