ebook img

Appetitive odor learning does not change olfactory coding in a subpopulation of honeybee PDF

21 Pages·2011·4.53 MB·English
by  
Save to my drive
Quick download
Download
Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.

Preview Appetitive odor learning does not change olfactory coding in a subpopulation of honeybee

Appetitive odor learning does not change olfactory coding in a subpopulation of honeybee antennal lobe neurons P. Peele . M. Ditzen . R. Menzel . C. G. Galizia Abstract Odors elicit spatio-temporal patterns of modified by learning in this paradigm. The role that activity in the olfactory bulb of vertebrates and the other projection neurons play in olfactory learning antennal lobe of insects. There have been several re remains to be investigated. ports of changes in these patterns following olfactory learning. These studies pose a conundrum: how can an Keywords Learning and memory' Olfactory coding· animal learn to efficiently respond to a particular od or Calcium imaging . Projection neurons . Coding with an adequate response, if its primary representa invariance tion already changes during this process? In this study, we offer a possible solution for this problem. We Abbreviations measured odor-evoked calcium responses in a sub AL Antennal lobe population of uniglomerular AL output neurons in CR Conditioned response honeybees. We show that their responses to odors are CS- Non-rewarded stimulus remarkably resistant to plasticity following a variety of CS+ Rewarded (conditioned) stimulus appetitive olfactory learning paradigms. There was no ITI Inter-trial interval significant difference in the changes of odor-evoked I-ACT Lateral antenno-cerebralis tract activity between single and multiple trial forward or LP Lateral protocerebrum backward conditioning, differential conditioning, or m-ACT Medial antenno-cerebralis tract unrewarded successive odor stimulation. In a behav MB Mushroom body ioral learning experiment we show that these neurons ml-ACT Mediolateral antenno-cerebralis tract are necessary for conditioned odor responses. We OB Olfactory bulb conclude that these uniglomerular projection neurons OSN Olfactory sensory neuron are necessary for reliable odor coding and are not PN Projection neuron RMANOVA Repeated measures analysis of variance SEG Subesophageal ganglion Tl-T4 Tracts 1-4 in the antennallobe P. PeeIe . M. Ditzen . R. MenzelC. G. Galizia Institute of Neurobiology, Freie Universitat Berlin, US Unconditioned stimulus 14195 Berlin, Germany PER Proboscis extension reflex VUMmx1 Ventral unpaired neuron # 1 of the C. G. Galizia maxillary neuromere Department of Entomology, University of California, Riverside, CA 92521, USA Introduction Present Address: C. G. Galizia ([2;]) The internal representation of the external world is Universitat Konstanz, 78457 Konstanz, Germany e-mail: [email protected] an inherent and fundamental function of all nervous 1084 systems that needs to be reliable to ensure the gener multiple conditioning trials induce a stable, long-term ation of adequate behavioral responses. This is also memory that needs both translation and transcription true for the chemical senses, which play a vital role for (MenzeI1999). most animals. Primary olfactory information is pro The cellular changes leading to learning appear to be cessed in the vertebrate olfactory bulb (OB) and its dispersed among different areas of the brain (Menzel insect analogue, the antennal lobe (AL) (Hildebrand 1990; Gluck and Granger 1993; Tully et al. 1994; Ham and Shepherd 1997). Both are subdivided into func mer and Menzel 1998; MenzeI1999). Amongst others, tional processing units, the olfactory glomeruli. Odors neural plasticity due to olfactory learning has been elicit specific spatio-temporal combinatorial patterns of found at the level of the first processing station in the activated glomeruli in vertebrate OBs and invertebrate olfactory pathway, in the vertebrate OB (Woo et al. ALs (Galizia and Menzel 2001; Friedrich 2002). A 1987; Sullivan and Wilson 1995; 10hnson et al. 1995; particular glomerulus receives afferent input by a Brennan et al. 1998) and the insect AL (Faber et al. specific olfactory sensory neuron type (OSN). Local 1999; Sandoz et al. 2003; Yu et al. 2004; Daly et al. 2004). neurons within the OB or the AL modify the incoming Changes at the first odor processing level are likely to activity (Mal un 1991; Yokoi et al. 1995; Mori et al. affect the neural representation of an odor. But how can 1999; Sachse and Galizia 2002), and the processed an animal learn to respond to a particular odor if its information is relayed to higher order brain centers by neural representation is changing? Moreover, behav vertebrate mitral/tufted cells or the insect projection ioral phenomena such as contextual, structural and neurons (PNs). Higher order olfactory neuropiles in configural learning require a stable representation of insects are the mushroom bodies (MB) and the lateral stimulus features (for example, that of an odor) in dif pro to cerebrum (LP) (Mobbs 1982; Abel et al. 2001; ferent circuits that are selectively and specifically com Muller et al. 2002; Marin et al. 2002; Wong et al. 2002; bined (Gerber and Menzel 2000; Sandoz and Menzel Tanaka et al. 2004). In the honeybee, OSN afferents 2001; Menzel and Giurfa 2001; Deisig et al. 2001). We travel in four different tracts, T1-T4, that innervate therefore investigated the effects of associative learning about 160 glomeruli altogether. PNs run in three dif on the representation of olfactory information in the ferent antenno-cerebral tracts (ACT). The medio-Iat AL. To this end, we combined a variety of appetitive eral ACT (ml-ACT) contains pluriglomerular cells. olfactory conditioning paradigms with in vivo calcium The lateral and median ACTs (1-and m-ACTs) contain imaging of one subpopulation of the olfactory PNs, the 1- axons of uniglomerular PNs (Abel et al. 2001). It ACT PNs (Sachse and Galizia 2002). We measured odor should be noted that the nomenclature for PN tracts responses before, during and after associative olfactory differs for different insect species. For example, the learning, and found that od or representation in these location of the honeybee I-ACT corresponds to the cells is reliable and stable throughout appetitive learn Drosophila oACT, and whether the two are homolo ing experiments. gous remains to be investigated. Vertebrates as well as many invertebrates can be conditioned to olfactory stimuli (Menzel 1990; Wilson Methods and Stevenson 2003; Davis 2004). Sucrose stimulation of antennal or proboscis chemoreceptors of a hungry Preparation bee leads to the proboscis extension response (PER). In an appetitive olfactory learning paradigm (PER Adult foraging bees, Apis melli/era carnica, were conditioning), an odor, the conditioned stimulus (CS), caught at the hive in the afternoon 1 day before the is paired with a subsequent sucrose reward as the experiments. Bees were expcrimentally naIve, but may unconditioned stimulus (US). The animals form an have experienced odors during foraging flights. They association between the two, so that an odor stimula were cooled for anesthesia, fixed in a recording stage tion alone elicits the PER (conditioned response, CR) (Galizia et al. 1997), and fed with sucrose water (30%) previously elicited only by the US. This effect is clearly until satiation. Bees were kept at 20°C in a dark associative and involves classical conditioning (Bit humidified box over night. The next day PNs were terman et al. 1983). The number of conditioning trials stained as follows: a window was cut in the head cap applied to the honeybee influences which kind of sule and glands and trachea were carefully removed to memory is induced. A single conditioning trial results allow visual access to the brain. A glass electrode was in short-term memory which decays rapidly and is coated with crystals of FURA-dextran (potassium salt, sensitive to amnesic treatments. This memory is inde 10.000 MW, Invitrogen, Germany), dissolved in 3% pendent of translation and transcription. In contrast, bovine serum albumin solution (Albumin Fraction V, 1085 AppliChem GmbH, Germany), and injected into the using a computer controlled olfactometer (Galizia right deutocerebrum dorsolateral to the alpha-lobe, et al. 1997). aiming for the I-ACT. Using different activity indica For the US we delivered a drop of 2 fll 30% aqueous tors including calcium dyes, other studies have suc sucrose solution to the antennae and the proboscis, cessfully demonstrated plasticity in neural signa ling in using a 10 fll pipette with standard tips. vertebrates and invertebrates (Faber et al. 1999; Faber and Menzel 2001; Yu et al. 2004; Mutoh et al. 2005; Data analysis Lohmann and Wong 2005). This indicates that calcium sensitive dyes do not interfere with the intracellular All data were analyzed using custom software written signaling cascades to a degree that would prevent any in IDL (Research systems, CO, USA). First, the raw modulatory effects. The dye was left to travel along the fluorescent images were manually corrected for tract for 4-8 h. Then, bees were prepared by fixing the movement within each measurement and for shifts antennae with soft dental wax (Kerr, Sybron Dental between measurements. Scattered light correction was Special ties, USA). Animals were immobilized by fixing applied to avoid that glomeruli with strong activity the thorax and abdomen to the recording chamber with would cause fictive activity in neighboring silent wax (Deiberit 502, Dr Boehme&Schoeps, Germany). glomeruli (Galizia and Vetter 2004). We calculated The brain was rinsed with Ringer solution (130 mM F/ = Fi + n[Fi - sm(Fj)], where Fi is image number i, NaCl,6 mM KCl, 4 mM MgCl2, 5 mM CaCl2, 160 mM and sm(F;) the same image after application of a spatial sucrose, 25 mM glucose, 10 mM HEPES, pH 6.7, low-pass filter. We used a boxcar average filter with 500 mOsmol; all chemicals from Sigma-Aldrich, Ger kernel size of 20 flm, i.e. less than the radius of one many). Strong movements of the brain needed to be glomerulus. The images F/ are the scattered-light prevented to achieve stable imaging experiments. corrected images. The number n gives the strength of Therefore, a second hole was cut ventrally to the the applied filter. In this study, we found n :::: 3 to give antennae and the compact structure of muscles, the best results, as compared to n = 1 in previous work esophagus, and supporting chitin was lifted and put (Galizia and Vetter 2004). Note that this procedure under slight tension (Mauelshagen 1993). Thus stabil does not change the measured values in areas where ization was accomplished without damage to the brain. there is no spatial contrast: in these areas, the low However, this preparation impairs the PER. To allow passed filtered image has the same value as the original for sucrose stimulation the proboscis was manually image, and consequently F;' = Fi• extended and placed on a small glass capillary that was For each image i, we then calculated the percentage fixed to the recording chamber. After surgery and be ratio Ri = (Fi 340nmlFi 380nm) X 100. Background ratio fore recording the bees were left to recover for at least was determined by an average of five frames obtained 30 min. before stimulation and was subtracted from every ratio frame of a measurement, thereby setting ratio values to o Imaging just before stimulus onset. The resulting values are proportional to intracellular calcium concentration Imaging was done using a CCD based imaging system changes, but are measured in arbitrary units, since we (Polychrome IV with Imago-QE camera, T.I.L.L. Pho could not calibrate absolute calcium concentration. tonics, Germany). Monochromatic excitation light The morphological structure allowed us to identify alternated between 340 and 380 nm. Fluorescence was glomeruli on the basis of their borderlines using the detected at a sampling rate of 5 Hz with a fluorescence digital atlas of the AL as a reference (Galizia et al. microscope (Olympus BX-50WI, Japan) equipped with 1999a). This method has already been described in de a 20x, NA = 0.95 dip objective (Olympus), 505 tail (Sachse et al. 1999; Galizia et al. 1999b). Honeybee DRLPXR dichroic mirror and 515 nm LP filter (Omega glomeruli are labeled with a number and the name of the Filters, VT, USA). Resolution was 172 x 130 pixels antennal nerve tract that innervates them, ranging from binned on chip from 1,376 x 1,040 pixels, resulting in a Tl to T4, e.g. Tl-28 or T3-45 (Flanagan and Mercer spatial sampling rate of 2.4 flm per pixel side. 1989). Since all glomeruli in this study were from the T1 Odors were diluted in mineral oil to 1 % (l-hexanol) tract, we have simplified their names by omitting T1 or 0.1 % (l-octanol and 1-nonanol, all odors from Sig from the name. We analyzed responses in glomeruli 17, ma-Aldrich, Germany). Five microliter of the od or 23, 25, 28, 29, 33, 35, 36, 37, 38, 42, 47, 48, 49 and 60. solution were applied onto a 1 cm2 piece of filter paper "Glomerular activity" in this study specifically refers to and placed in a plastic syringe. Odors were injected activity of the uniglomerular PNs in each glomerulus, into a continuous air stream directed to the antennae because only these neurons were stained, and therefore 1086 only this particular population of cells was measured in 3bw 3-trial backward conditioning with ITI of 2 min each glomerulus. For each identified glomerulus time (n = 11) courses were averaged taking an area of 9 x 9 pixels 3+ 3-trial forward conditioning with ITI of 2 min (corresponding to 21.6 !lm x 21.6 !lm always within the (n = 11). glomerulus chosen). Glomerular calcium response magnitude (hereafter simply "response") was calcu This nomenclature indicates whether I-nonanol was lated as the mean of the time course during odor stim unrewarded (-), forward paired with sucrose water (+) ulation. Statistical analysis was performed using or backward paired with sucrose water (bw). The Sigmastat (SPSS Inc, IL, USA). number (1, 3) corresponds to how often the condi For false-color display, responses were calculated tioning trials were repeated. for each pixel. The resulting image was median filtered Calcium responses for each glomerulus were ana in space (3 x 3 pixels). Iyzed with a two-way repeated measures (RM) ANO V A, with the different conditioning groups as one Plasticity of odor responses factor and the "pre" -responses and "post" -responses at different points in time as the repeated factor. All experimental protocols are summarized in Fig. 1. 1- Multiple comparisons were performed using the Holms-Sidak post-hoc test. nonanol was always the reinforced odor (CS). In one part of the experiments (set 1) we investigated changes of the responses to 1-nonanol due to repeated stimula Multiple od or experiments tions, and due to pairing of this od or with a sucrose reward in an absolute conditioning paradigm, with We investigated the effect of sequential stimulation special focus on progression in time after the condi with three odors and differential conditioning (Fig. 1, tioning phase. Thereafter, we included I-hexanol and 1- set 2). Each experiment consisted of measuring od or octanol into the stimulation protocols, allowing us to responses before conditioning ("pre"), conditioning, examine the effects of absolute conditioning on the and measuring odor responses after conditioning representation of non-conditioned odors and the effect ("post"), similar to experiments of set 1. "Pre" and of differential conditioning on all odors involved (set 2). "post" consisted of three blocks with three odor stimulations each (l-hexanol, l-octanol and I-nona Single odor experiments nol). Within "pre" and "post" blocks the odor se quence was pseudorandomized to prevent odor We investigated the effects of absolute conditioning on sequence effects (for example, an od or sequence could the neural representation of the odor 1-nonanol be ABC, CAB, BCA, with each odor at a first, a second (Fig. 1, set 1). The experiments consisted of three and a third position within the blocks). Different ani parts: od or responses before conditioning ("pre"), mals were tested with different sequences. We aver conditioning, and odor responses after conditioning aged the three "pre" responses and the three "post" ("post"). Odor stimulation was always 3 s long. We responses to yield one "pre" and one "post" response determined the "naive" response to I-nonanol in the to every odor which we then compared. CS-US pairing "pre"-phase during which all animals received two was done as for absolute conditioning (see above). presentations of 1-nonanol with an inter-trial interval Stimulus sequence within the conditioning block was (ITI) of 2 min. During conditioning a sucrose reward identical for all animals within each group. was applied for 3 s to the ipsilateral antenna of the Seven groups were subject to the following condi imaged AL and to the proboscis. For forward pairing tioning protocols: the odor preceded the reinforcement, for backward 3N+ 3-trial absolute conditioning, ITI of 2 min pairing that order was reversed, the overlap was 1 s in (no CS-) (n = 10) both cases. During the "post" phase all animals were 3N+O- 3-trial differential conditioning, ITI of 1 min, presented with I-nonanol after 1, 5 and 15 min fol CS+ 1-nonanol, CS- l-octanol (n = 9) lowing conditioning. 5N+ 5-trial absolute conditioning, ITI of 2 min Five groups were subject to the different condi (no CS-) (n = 15) tioning protocols: 5N+O- 5-trial differential conditioning, ITI of 1 min, r 1-trial unrewarded I-nonanol (n = 9) CS+ I-nonanol, CS- l-octanol (n = 9) lbw 1-trial backward conditioning (n = 14) 5N+H- 5-trial differential conditioning, CS+ 1- = 1+ I-trial forward conditioning (n 9) nonanol, CS- 1-hexanol (n = 10) 1087 Fig. 1 Stimulus protocols of pre conditioning post the experiments. Stimulus duration for odors and sDuucrrionsge pwaaisri naglw, athyse 3tw so. 1- I• • I• . L ... 1min ... \ ~ . ~, \ I, . stimuli overlapped for 1 s. bw Grersopuecpt ilvaeb eclos nrdeiftlieocnt inthge - 1bw I. • I• • L ... 1min ... \ ~ • ~\ \ I. , ,... protocol. The number gives + throiawl wofatse nre tpheea tceodn.d (it-i)o nstianngd s eCnl) 1+ I, . .I L ... 1min ... \ ~ • ~1 \ .I , fafoonrrd w u(anbrrwde )wp faaoirrrdi nebdga cowkdiwtohar ,rs du( +c)r ofsoer 3bw I. • I• , ,b"w I • bwI• •b wIi ,I ... 1min ... , ~ • ~1 \ I. . pairing. 1-Nonanol was the + + + othnely f iorsdt ofri vgei vgerno utphsr o(usgeth o1u) to f 3+ I. • I• • \ .I. I• • ~\ ... 1min ... \ ~ • ~\ \ I, . -4 -3 -2 -1 5 15 the experiments. For the last seven groups (set 2), odors are indicated by capital .I+. I+ I+, 1 I 0 I 0 I 0 I I letters: 1-hexanol (H), 3N+ ~~p~p~p~~\ \ • • 1\ ... 5m~ "',• i i I , i • i I i , l-octanol (0) and 1-nonanol (N). Stimulations of the three 11 0 I 0 I 0 I I, + + + p p p . odors were randomized in the 3WO' \~~~~~\ ... 5min ... \ ~ ~ ~ ~ ~ ~ i i i i i • i i i \ "pre" and "post" phase + + + + + 110101011, I I I I I 11 0 I 0 I 0 I I Nd) 5N+ i • i i i i i i i , , •••••••••• I.. 5min ".\ ••••••• , •• en ~ ~ ~ ~ ~ ~ ~ ~ ~ ~ ~ ~ p~ p~ p~ ~ . 5N+O' ~~p~p~p~~\ \ 1. . 5min ... \ 5N+H' ~~p~p~p~~\ \ ~ P ~ P ~ P~ ~ ~ ~ ~ ~ p~ p~ p~ ~ . 1. . 5min ... \ 01 I I I 0 I 0 I, p p p . 5N'O- "~ ~ ~ ~ ~ ~ ~ ~ ~ ~ ~ ~ ~ ~ ~ i i i I • i i i i I , \ .. 5min ... \ P~ ~ ~ ~ p~ p~ \ \~ P~ P ~ ~ ~ ~ ~ ~ \.. ~ ~ p~ p~ p~ ~ , 5NH 5min ... \ -9 ·8 -7 -6 -5 -4 -3 -2 -1 5 6 7 8 9 1011 1213 time (min) time (min) • 1-nonanol III!II 1-octanol 0 1-hexanol + = bw= backward pairing forward pairing 5N-0- 5-trial stimulation as in 5N+0- but without ("post"), including all three odors I-hexanol, l-octanol any reward (n = 10) and 1-nonanol. 5N-H- 5-trial stimulation as in 5N+H- but without Odor responses to the three odors in every glo any reward (n = 9). merulus were analyzed with a two-way RM ANOVA, with the different conditioning groups as one factor In this nomenclature, the number (3, 5) indicates how and "pre"-responses and "post"-responses as the re often conditioning trials were repeated. Conditioned peated factor time. Multiple comparisons were per odors are indicated by capital letters: 1-hexanol (H), 1- formed using the Holms-Sidak post-hoc test. octanol (0) and 1-nonanol (N), with a (+) for rein forced odor or a (-) for unrewarded presentation. Behavior Thus, 5N+0- represents a 5-trial differential condi tioning, with 1-nonanol reinforced and l-octanol The animals were prepared and stained as in the unrewarded. imaging experiments, with the difference that the Five minutes after conditioning the animals were esophagus and the muscles around the proboscis were subject to olfactory stimulation as described above not restrained in order to allow for the PER. First, 1088 animals were tested for PER to sucrose stimulation at on both antennae (data not shown). This indicates that each antenna, i.e. both the ipsilateral and the contra no bilateral neurons contributed to our signal, notably lateral antenna to the stained I-ACT PNs. Learning also not the VUMmxl neuron. Most likely, these neu experiments started 30 min later to avoid any effects rons were not labeled with the calcium sensitive dye. due to sensitization (Menzel et al. 1991). Odor stimulation with I-nonanol was again 3 s, sim PN responses to sucrose are similar ilar to the absolute conditioning experiments. Intertrial between animals interval was 2 min throughout "pre" and conditioning. All animals received two "pre" odor stimulations. In addition to odors, PNs also responded to sugar Thereafter the animals were conditioned by a 3-trial water stimulus at the antenna. In Fig. 3 two typical absolute conditioning as described above. Five minutes examples of an odor stimulation paired with a sucrose after conditioning the animals were tested for PER to 1- US are shown, one forward (Fig. 3a) and one backward nonanol stimulation at the ipsilateral antenna, at the (Fig. 3b). Sucrose solution evoked an activity pattern contralateral antenna and at both antennae. The order which was consistent across animals, and identical to of stimulation was randomized between animals. Stim stimulation with water alone. The response pattern ulation of only one antenna was achieved by covering consisted of several activated glomeruli, suggesting a the other antenna with a sealed plastic tube. All animals combinatorial representation as known for odors. The were then examined for stained I-ACT PNs under a response pattern to water and sucrose solution in fluorescent microscope. Only successfully stained ani cluded glomeruli that also respond to volatile odors mals were included in the analysis. (such as glomerulus 17) and glomeruli for which no volatile odor is yet known as a suitable stimulus (such as glomerulus 42). The strongest activity was in glo Results merulus 42 and the lowest was in 33 (Fig. 3c). PNs responded only to sucrose solution when stimulated on Odors elicit activity in PNs in the same glomeruli the ipsilateral antennae, but not when stimulated at the across animals contralateral antenna or at the proboscis (data not shown). Sucrose responses were clearly distinguishable Insertion of FURA-dextran crystals into the I-ACT from mechanical stimulation (data not shown). nerve tract led to a selective staining of PNs (Fig. 2a), as previously reported (Sachse and Galizia 2002). We Repeated odor stimulation does not change identified a set of 15 glomeruli in each of the 126 ani its representation mals included in this study using the digital three dimensional atlas of the honeybee antennal lobe All 54 animals of set 1 (see Fig. 1) received two "pre" (Galizia et al. 1999a) (Fig. 2a, c). nonanol presentations. PN odor responses in glomer When stimulating with I-hexanol, l-octanol or 1- ulus 17 and 33 across animals were indistinguishable nonanol, PNs showed the same glomerular spatio for the two "pre" stimuli (Fig. 4a). The same held true temporal actlVlty patterns published previously for all other glomeruli (RM two-way ANOVA , Holms (Fig. 2b, d) (Sachse et al. 1999; Sachse and Galizia Sidak multiple comparisons, smallest P = 0.059 for 2002). Hexanol evoked a strong calcium response in glomerulus 17, all other P > 0.1). glomerulus 28, and weaker responses in glomerulus 17, It is necessary to know the response variability to 36, 38 and 35, whereas calcium concentration in glo repeated odor stimulation alone before investigating merulus 29 decreased (Fig. 2e). Octanol elicited strong learning effects. Six successive nonanol stimulations responses in glomeruli 17 and 28, weak activity in (IN-group, Fig. 4b) over the 21 min of the experiment glomerulus 33 and a Ca2+ decrease in glomeruli 23, 25 elicited responses that remained unchanged in both and 29. Nonanol evoked a strong response in glomeruli magnitude (AN OVA) and shape (visual inspection) in 17 and 33 and a Ca2+ decrease in glomeruli 23 and 25. all glomeruli (see responses to glomeruli 17 and 33 in All responses were consistent across animals, as can be Fig. 4c). seen by the small error bars (Fig. 2e). The responses to octanol were overlapping with those to nonanol, while Absolute conditioning does not change the response to hexanol showed virtually no overlap of the representation of the learned odor activity with nonanol in the 15 observed glomeruli. No responses could be measured in the AL contra The glomerular odor responses were not statistically lateral to the staining side when stimulating with odors different at any point in time (time) between the dif- 1089 Fig. 2 Odor responses in the B 1-nonanol l-octanol 1-hexanol honeybee AL. a Raw A 11 fluorescence image of an AL stained with FURA. b False color coded spatial response patterns to I-hexanol, 1- octanol and I-nonanol. -2 Glomerulus 17,28 and 33 are circled with a dOlled white line. c Schematic view of the 1-nonanol (n = 72) l-octanol (n = 72) 1-hexanol (n = 72) AL with the 15 identified C D glomeruli used in this study. 14 14 14 12 12 12 17 d Mean time traces of odor 10 10 10 responses in selected .r 28 glomeruli (n = 72 animals). N ro 29 Time trace colors correspond a~: 33 to glomeruli in c. Stimulus is 0 47 marked by the shaded area. ·2 ·2 ·2 e Odor responses to ·4 ·2·10123456 ·4 ·I2-·r1- -0f' -~1 -2' 3. -4 ~5 ~6 ·4 ·I2-·r1- -0f' -~1 2 -'3. -~4 ~5 6 I-nonanol, l-octanol and lime(s) I-hexanol for 15 glomeruli E 12 (± SEM, n = 72, average during the 3 s odor stimulation). The three most 10 • 1-nonanol responsive glomeruli for these odors are indicated by colored 8 1-octanol squares (eJn) D 1-hexanol c 0 6 0en. . ~ 0 4 "0 0 2 n = 72 -2 [22]13311281 36 38 35 47 48 42 25 60 49 37 23 29 glomeruli c maximum response to sucrose A forward pairing B backward pairing (n = 20) 20 20 1-non8nol 15 15 10 10 5 5 o o -5 -5 -10 +-.-,--.--.-,s_u,,-ga,...r-,--,--,-, -10 +-,--.-,s-,ug,,-a,...r-.--.--,,--r-.- -2 -1 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 -2 -1 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 17 33 28 36 38 35 47 48 42 25 60 49 37 23 29 time (s) glomeruli Fig. 3 PN responses to sucrose and nonanol. Time traces in five marked by a shaded area, sucrose is marked by the yellow bar. glomeruli of an individual measurement for forward pairing (a) c Averages of maximum responses for 15 identified glomeruli and backward pairing (b). The odor-stimulus (nonanol) is during sucrose stimulation (± SEM, n = 20) 1090 Fig. 4 Unrewarded odor A glomerulus 17 (n = 56) glomerulus 33 (n = 56) responses are stable over time. a Calcium responses in 12 3j10 12 3j10 c 1st 2nd c 1st 2nd two identified glomeruli, 17 10 8.. B 10 8.. B and 33, to the first and second C/l C/l 1ti-mnoe ntarancoel sotfi malul l5a4ti oann im(maelsa on f N. ror 86 "0@00 4 86 "o@8 64 set 1). The odor-stimulus is (e)r 4 4 marked by a shaded area. 2 2 IT! = 2 min. Inset shows 0 0 average odor responses (± SEM, n = 54) during -2 -2 stimulation. b Group r , -4 -4 average responses ( ± SEM, -2 -1 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 -2 -1 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 n = 9) of 15 identified time (5) glomeruli to six consecutive 1- nonanol stimulations. C B group 1 (n=9) Average time traces (group 10 r) for glomeruli 17 and 33 • 8 -4' • -2' 0' 11 IcCD/ l 6 l' 0 0 5' 0 @CC/ll . 4 15' 0 0 "0 0 2 -2 17 33 28 36 38 35 47 48 42 25 60 49 37 23 29 glomeruli C glomerulus 17, group 1-(n = 9) glomerulus 33, group 1-(n= 9) 12 12 -4' -- 10 10 -2' ........ 0' ___ 8 8 l' .r 6 6 5' '"ro 4 4 15' Q 0:: 2 2 0 0 -2 -2 -4 -4 -2 -1 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 -2 -1 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 time (5) ferent stimulus protocols (group) in set 1 (Fig. 1), i.e. ing-independent way, i.e. without a statistical successive odor stimulation, single and multiple trial significance for learning paradigm, but common across conditioning, backward pairing and forward pairing all groups. Such changes are correlated with experi (Fig. 5) (no significant interaction between time and mental time, and not with treatment. Indeed, pooling group, RM two-way ANOV A, lowest P for glomerulus all animals, we found significant differences in some 17: df= 16, F = 1.614, P = 0.067, in all other cases odor responses. Responses to nonanol at 1 min after P > 0.3). Therefore, absolute appetitive conditioning training were attenuated in glomerulus 17 (Holms-Si did not change the neural representation of the learned dak, P = 0.001) and the inhibition in glomerulus 38 was odor in I-ACT projection neurons. reduced (P = 0.024); at 15 min, odor responses in glo Even in the absence of learning-related modula merulus 38 showed less inhibition (P = 0.001), whereas = tions, odor responses might be modulated in a learn- glomeruli 36 (P = 0.015) and 42 (P 0.03) were 1091 • Fig. 5 Neural representation glomerulus 17 • remains stable following glomerulus 17 glomerulus 33 glomerulus 33 sucrose reinforcement. PN response traces of glomerulus 1- 12 12 10 c1o7 nadnidti o3n3i ntog 1(-"npornea"n) ol before (n = 9) ~ 8 8 <8cQn) . 86 compared with responses "Q'", 4 4 <e?n 4 ("post") at 1,5 and 15 min er: 0 0 -(0; 2 following conditioning. The 0 third column shows the ·4 0 -2 0 2 4 6 8 10 0 2 4 6 8 10 pre l' 5' 15' average odor responses of time (s) glomerulus 17 and 33 (± SEM, n is given in each 1+ 12 12 10 graph) Q) 8 8 8 i!! (n = 14) + 8. 6 '"Q' " 4 4 ~ <e?n 4 er: (; 0 0 -0 2 0 .' -4 ·4 0 -2 0 2 4 6 8 10 -2 0 2 4 6 8 10 pre l' 5' 15' time (s) 1b w 12 12 8 Q) 8 8 i!! 6 (n=9) r a0 . "Ua'" ,: 4 4 ~ 4 (; 0 ..... 0 -0 2 0 -4 0 -2 0 2 4 6 8 10 0 2 4 6 8 10 pre l' 5' 15' time (s) 3+ 12 12 10 8 8 c~ 8 (n=11) r 8. 6 "Q'", 4 4 <e?n 4 er: (; 0 0 -0 2 0 -4 -4 0 -2 0 2 4 6 8 10 -2 0 2 4 6 8 10 pre l' 5' 15' time (s) 3bw 12 12 10 8 8 c~ 8 (n=11)r 8. 6 "ua'":, 4 4 \ <e?n 4 0 o ~ \ -.:...J".:' ". ~;j,',: .'{:f,-,v-. :' -(00; 2 ·4 -4 0 -2 0 2 4 6 8 10 -2 0 2 4 6 8 10 pre l' 5' 15' time(s) slightly increased (data not shown). All other glomeruli ory induction (Muller 2000b). Three-trial backward remained unchanged compared to the "pre" at all conditioning, however, does not lead to learning nor to "post" time points (RM two-way ANOVA ). a PKA increase. Therefore, we investigated the re sponses to the CS-US pairing during mUltiple trial Both forward and backward pairing leave the conditioning. When comparing forward (group 3+) learned odor unchanged with backward conditioning (group 3bW; Fig. 6), it was not possible to compare responses to the CS only, be While single trial conditioning leads to short-term cause CS and US overlap in different ways. For memory only, 3-trial forward pairing leads to long-term example, during the first second of the CS stimulation learning and to a prolonged elevation of PKA activity the backward group already includes the US (group in the AL which is necessary for this long-term mem- 3hw), while the forward group includes the US only 1092 during the last second (group 3+) (Fig. 3). We there Appetitive conditioning does not change responses fore analyzed the combined response to CS and US to multiple other odors (set 2) during the conditioning trial (see Methods). Com pound responses to the CS and US did not differ be Having established that olfactory conditioning does tween the two paradigms (smallest P for glomerulus 48 not change the representation of the conditioned odor, with df = 1, F = 1.972, P = 0.176 two-way RM ANO we wondered whether other odors might be modified V A). There was no difference between successive trials by learning. To this end, we tested odor responses to (smallest P for glomerulus 25 with df = 2, F = 1.399, three odors before and after training, and conditioned P = 0.259) and no significant interaction between the nonanol alone or differentially against octanol or paradigms and trials (smallest P for glomerulus 29 with hexanol (set 2, Fig. 1). Statistical analysis of the three df = 2, F = 1.441, P = 0.295), with onc exception: "prc" odor responses and the three "post" odor re Glomerulus 60 showed a difference in trial (df = 2, sponses revealed no significant differences within F = 3.355, P = 0.045) and a Holms-Sidak multiple "pre" or within "post" (ANOVA for all odor re comparison revealed that trial 2 was different from sponses in each glomerulus, df = 2, smallest P = 0.079). both, trial 1 and trial 3 (P = 0.03, traces not shown). For statistical analysis, therefore, the average of all There was no significant interaction between trial and three responses preceding the treatment was defined as paradigm (P = 0.301) and in both groups the second the "pre" odor response for every odor and likewise trial was slightly reduced. Glomerulus 60 neither for the "post" odor response. showed a pronounced response to nonanol nor to su There was no significant effect of the conditioning crose stimulation. We therefore regard this significant paradigms (group) on any of the od or responses (time) value as a statistical effect typical for multiple com (i.e. the CS+, the CS-or the neutral odor; RM two-way parison studies and as biologically not relevant. ANOVA , smallest P with df = 6, F = 2.152, P = 0.059, all other P > 0.1). There was no significant interaction between group and time. In more detail, the different A backward, group 3bW (n=11) paradigms gave the following results. Odor response 8 D for the CS+ and the neutral odors were unchanged trial 1 Cl after five trials of absolute conditioning (group 5N+; trial 2 III trial 3 Fig. 7, compare with 3+, Fig. 5), as shown for the responsive glomeruli 17,28 and 33 in Fig. 7. Differential conditioning of nonanol against another odor also did not induce any changes in the responses of any of the odors involved. Responses to CS+, CS-or the neutral od or (groups 5N+O-and 5N+H-, Fig. 8) did not change, even for odors with highly overlapping -2 17 33 28 36 38 35 47 48 42 25 60 49 37 23 29 glomerular patterns (group 5N+O-). Responses did glomeruli also not change in those groups which received the B forward, group 3+ (n=11) same odor sequence but without rewarding nonanol (groups 5WO- and 5N-H-, Fig. 8). Even if od or responses are unchanged after condi tioning as compared to before conditioning, odor re sponses might change during the repeated learning trials. In this analysis it is important to also include CS+and CS-, because in behavioral studies of differ ential conditioning the first reinforcement of the CS+ leads to an enhanced response to the subsequent CS-, even though the CS- is not paired with the US, indi 17 33 28 36 38 35 47 48 42 25 60 49 37 23 29 cating that in this condition animals generalize be glomeruli tween CS+ and CS-. As differential training Fig. 6 Responses are stable during conditioning. The compound progresses, the response to the CS- returns to the response to 1-nonanol and sucrose is not modulated during spontaneous response level, or falls below it (Bit multiple trial conditioning. a Average compound responses terman et aI. 1983). We therefore compared the CS+ (± SEM, n = 11) during trial 1,2 and 3 of backward conditioning (group 3bw). b The same for forward conditioning (group 3+, and CS- responses during conditioning (where gener n = 11) alization would be expected) with the respective

Description:
animal learn to efficiently respond to a particular od or Yokohari F, Tominaga Y, Tateda H (1982) Antennal hygrore- ceptors of the honey bee, k.
See more

The list of books you might like

Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.