ebook img

An Old Melody in a New Song: Aesthetics and the Art of Psychology PDF

232 Pages·2018·4.037 MB·English
Save to my drive
Quick download
Download
Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.

Preview An Old Melody in a New Song: Aesthetics and the Art of Psychology

Theory and History in the Human and Social Sciences Luca Tateo Editor An Old Melody in a New Song Aesthetics and the Art of Psychology Theory and History in the Human and Social Sciences Series Editor Jaan Valsiner Department of Communication and Psychology Aalborg University Aalborg, Denmark Theory and History in the Human and Social Sciences will fill in the gap in the existing coverage of links between new theoretical advancements in the social and human sciences and their historical roots. Making that linkage is crucial for the interdisciplinary synthesis across the disciplines of psychology, anthropology, sociology, history, semiotics, and the political sciences. In contemporary human sciences of the 21st there exists increasing differentiation between neurosciences and all other sciences that are aimed at making sense of the complex social, psychological, and political processes. Thus new series has the purpose of (1) coordinating such efforts across the borders of existing human and social sciences, (2) providing an arena for possible inter-disciplinary theoretical syntheses, (3) bring into attention of our contemporary scientific community innovative ideas that have been lost in the dustbin of history for no good reasons, and (4) provide an arena for international communication between social and human scientists across the World. More information about this series at http://www.springer.com/series/15826 Luca Tateo Editor An Old Melody in a New Song Aesthetics and the Art of Psychology Editor Luca Tateo Department of Communication and Psychology Centre for Cultural Psychology Aalborg University Aalborg, Denmark ISSN 2523-8663 ISSN 2523-8671 (electronic) Theory and History in the Human and Social Sciences ISBN 978-3-319-92338-3 ISBN 978-3-319-92339-0 (eBook) https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-92339-0 Library of Congress Control Number: 2018955627 © Springer International Publishing AG, part of Springer Nature 2018 This work is subject to copyright. All rights are reserved by the Publisher, whether the whole or part of the material is concerned, specifically the rights of translation, reprinting, reuse of illustrations, recitation, broadcasting, reproduction on microfilms or in any other physical way, and transmission or information storage and retrieval, electronic adaptation, computer software, or by similar or dissimilar methodology now known or hereafter developed. The use of general descriptive names, registered names, trademarks, service marks, etc. in this publication does not imply, even in the absence of a specific statement, that such names are exempt from the relevant protective laws and regulations and therefore free for general use. The publisher, the authors, and the editors are safe to assume that the advice and information in this book are believed to be true and accurate at the date of publication. Neither the publisher nor the authors or the editors give a warranty, express or implied, with respect to the material contained herein or for any errors or omissions that may have been made. The publisher remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. This Springer imprint is published by the registered company Springer Nature Switzerland AG The registered company address is: Gewerbestrasse 11, 6330 Cham, Switzerland The "I" who speaks in this book is by no means the author. Rather, the author wishes that the reader may come to see himself in this "I": that the reader may not simply relate to what is said here as he would to history, but rather that while reading he will actually converse with himself, deliberate back and forth, deduce conclusions, make decisions like his representative in the book, and through his own work and reflection, purely out of his own resources, develop and build within himself the philosophical disposition that is presented to him in this book merely as a picture. (p. 2) Fichte, J. G. (1848). The vocation of man. Trans W. Smith. London: John Chapman. Series Preface The Art of Science Being considered an art should be viewed as a compliment for any science, but it is (usually) not. This is particularly true in the case of psychology, which was first a casualty in the “science wars” in nineteenth-century Europe (Valsiner, 2012), fol- lowed by the skirmishes between emerging psychologists and hardline philosophers about the “dangers of psychologism” (Kusch, 1995) at the turn of the twentieth century. What followed over the next century were two World Wars; the exodus of psychology as a science from Europe to North America; and the establishment of psychology as “an empirical science.” In that transition, it took on new social orga- nizational forms from US society, which had embraced the arrival of this refugee science—the notion of “majority” (“mainstream” psychology) versus “minority” (various “nonmainstream” trends) . To speak about “mainstream” psychology in the year 1900 would have made no sense. Even if Wilhelm Wundt’s laboratory in Leipzig drew hordes of international visitors, who carried its handicrafts of labora- tory plans and design of experimental equipment all over the world, that laboratory was in no way the “mainstream” of psychology in Germany. One hundred years later, in 2000 and beyond, we are well-versed in referencing “mainstream psychol- ogy”—usually to contrast what we do ourselves positively against its dull intellec- tual modus operandi. I believe that such “mainstream bashing” is socially useless and intellectually dangerous. It is useless because the realities of research carried out by the “busy and mindless” ill-defined group of the “mainstream” are determined by the socio- political and economic setup conditions in institutions where research is done. These are shared with other sciences. Thus, it is often the conformity of fellow researchers—elevated to the minimal power role as reviewers of grants and other proposals—that maintains the “mindlessness” of the “mainstream” through simple administrative procedures. Thus, any critique of the “mainstream” would be socially mute and does not change the prevailing scientific–political practice. “Mainstream”- bashing is simply a waste of time. vii viii Series Preface Yet, the danger to intellectualism is more damaging—not to the “mainstream” but to its opponents themselves. The critique of the theoretical limitations of the “mainstream” channels the criticizers into repetitive argumentation and diminishes the possibilities for developing alternative positive perspectives. The latter are rather rare in contemporary psychology. Furthermore, what is offered as a solution for theoretical problems is a call to join a new fashion, which may be nicely labeled (e.g., “positive psychology,” “ecological psychology,” “evolutionary psychology,” and—to my serious concern—“cultural psychology”). If a discipline moves ahead by one fashion replacing another—rather than by theoretical efforts to innovate its conceptual core—it is only the never-ending flow of empirical research reports that follows. The “mainstream” wins again—after adopting a new label. All this becomes socially accepted by the model of science as that of accumulation of empirical data—“let us collect more data and the theoretical issues become resolved” is a prevailing ethos. The contributors to this volume take a different path—that of restoring the notion of artist-like exploration of psychological issues to the science of psychology. The volume includes work well informed in the history of aesthetic perspectives that grew out of philosophy and emerging psychology from its eighteenth-century roots. Thus, what is attempted here is building a new theoretical direction where the aes- thetic world view is the center of science, while tracing back to these foundational roots of psychology as an emerging science. It deserves careful attention from the readers—depending on how art in science is elaborated upon, it either leads to basic breakthroughs in psychology as a science, or becomes a new fashion that dies out before it has a chance to accomplish its goals. I, of course, hope for the former. Fashions do not develop ideas—they just pres- ent them. The first artful moment for a scientist is to distinguish between actual theoretical potentials for a breakthrough and fashion-based claims that such a break- through occurred, or is now happening, in the given field. Was there a breakthrough when the common notion of “thoughts” that satisfied psychologists in the early twentieth century were replaced by the fashionable notion of “cognitions” (note the plural here)? I have seen no benefit to the development of the understanding of the human mind (or “cognition”) in comparison to the efforts in that direction at the era of “thoughts” (e.g., the “Würzburg School” of Oswald Külpe in 1896–1909). However, the participants in this volume make an even more profound start— jumping from eighteenth-century aesthetics to twenty-first-century science. They do it under the contemporary up and coming (to fashion) label of “cultural psychol- ogy.” It really depends upon their success whether the label is rendered irrelevant by their innovation in the field—which, as they point out, starts from the need to inno- vate methodology. This innovation is no easy task. Methodology does not equal a “toolbox of meth- ods” that can be freely used, but an integrated system of epistemology (Branco and Valsiner, 1997). If that innovation were to begin from the meta-code “Science is a form of Art”—which it seems to do in this volume—it has major consequences for the ways in which psychologists as researchers work. They have to be interested in a personally disinterested way—the criterion of an aesthetic world view—in both Series Preface ix the most ordinary (Marsico & Tateo, 2018) and in the most extraordinary (Luria, 1987) phenomena. This is easier said than done—it is most perceptively pointed out (Devereux, 1967) that researchers are themselves complicated human beings whose own ego-defense mechanisms do not easily allow for the aesthetic relation to their object of study to develop. Psychologists’ educated intuitions are the result of for- mal schooling, administering procedures in research laboratories, and clinical con- sultation rooms; rarely would they be found building upon direct war-time experience (e.g., Bion, 1997). By contrast, artists immerse themselves in the “real life” in its unbounded fullness—on the battlefield (Vereshchagin), in the psychiatric hospital (van Gogh), or in brothels (Toulouse-Lautrec, Picasso, Kirschner, and many others), not to mention getting drunk and involved in restaurant and tavern outings. A drunken artist is a socially acceptable—even if not necessarily aestheti- cally pleasing—sight. A drunken psychologist administering an IQ test would be an unimaginably comic or grotesque scene if viewed as a thought experiment, and a breach in professional ethics if it occurred in reality. Despite the obvious difficulties by the researchers’ personal efforts to assume an aesthetic perspective, I believe it is possible. Yet, it would drive a division between theoretically oriented psychologists and those interested in social practice. The for- mer—from the distance of their ivory tower (read—crammed university office somewhere)—can reach the aesthetic criterion of feeling-in without feeling-about (interested disinterest), whereas for the social practice-oriented psychologists it would be very hard. Social practice requires involvement—which is contradictory to the aesthetic world view. The next arena for change is theory—the aesthetic perspective of research calls for replacement of both inductive and deductive knowledge-making tactics by their abductive counterpart. That requires artistic ingenuity in looking at the observed phenomena and imagining the various possible novel scenarios for its post factum explanations. In psychological theories one would not encounter an invented entity (“trait,” “heuristic,” etc.) suddenly moving to another trajectory (à la Niels Bohr’s physics) or expect it to be of two identities at the same time (e.g., androgyny—the simultaneous presence of male and female sides). There are no equivalents to the “jumping genes” (Barbara McClintock’s maize genetics) in psychology. This lack of ingenuity continues in the neurosciences, where already the complexity of the anatomy of the neuron should suggest otherwise. Finally, what would an aesthetic view of method construction look like? Psychologists’ methods have at times intuitively made use of the artistic general attitude by using drawing tasks to represent psychological phenomena. Projective techniques such as Rorschach’s inkblots and all kinds of narrative methods bring psychologists’ research tools very close to those of art. Yet, the closeness stops when the research participants have finished their tasks and the researchers begin “data analysis.” What is called “analysis” is actually an act of hybridization of the original products of the participants with superimposed—usually quantificational— data reconstruction schemes. The original phenomena get lost in the data, and they are never recovered. It is already for the sake of avoiding such loss that the new aesthetic start is promising in psychology. x Series Preface These challenges to the new aesthetic science in psychology are not fatal to the effort. Rather, they can stimulate the search for new solutions—often turning to re- organization of the current methods in methodologically sound ways. The present volume includes a number of promising directions for innovation in a co-i maginative effort to make psychology human, and possibly humane, again, and one I hope readers will join. Aalborg, Denmark Jaan Valsiner References Bion, W. R. (1997). War memoirs 1917–1919. London: Karnac Books. Branco, A. U., & Valsiner, J. (1997). Changing methodologies: a co-c onstructivist study of goal orientations in social interactions. Psychology and Developing Societies, 9, 1, 35–64. Devereux, G. (1967). From anxiety to method in the behavioral sciences. The Hague: Mouton Kusch, M. (1995). Psychologism: a case study in the sociology of philosophical knowledge. London: Routledge Luria, A. R. (1987). A mind of a mnemonist. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Marsico, G., & Tateo, L. (Eds) (2018). Ordinary things. Vol 3 in Annals of cultural psychology. Charlotte, NC: Information Age Publishers. Valsiner, J. (2012). A guided science: history of psychology in the mirror of its making. New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction Publishers.

See more

The list of books you might like

Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.