ebook img

Acoustic Communication in Birds. Production, Perception & Design Features of Sounds PDF

391 Pages·1983·6.729 MB·English
Save to my drive
Quick download
Download
Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.

Preview Acoustic Communication in Birds. Production, Perception & Design Features of Sounds

COMMUNICATION AND BEHAVIOR AN INTERDISCIPLINARY SERIES Under the Editorship of Duane M. Rumbaugh, Georgia State University and Yerkes Regional Primate Research Center off Emory University DUANE M. RUMBAUGH (ED.), LANGUAGE LEARNING BY A CHIMPANZEE: THE LANA PROJECT, 1977 ROBERTA L. HALL AND HENRY S. SHARP (EDS.), WOLF AND MAN: EVOLUTION IN PARALLEL, 1978 HORST D. STEKLIS AND MICHAEL J. RALEIGH (EDS.), NEUROBIOLOGY OF SOCIAL COMMUNICATION IN PRIMATES: AN EVOLUTIONARY PERSPECTIVE, 1979 P. CHARLES-DOMINIQUE, H. M. COOPER, A. HLADIK, C. M. HLADIK, E. PAGES, G. F. PARIENTE, A. PETTER-ROUSSEAUX, J. J. PETTER, AND A. SCHILLING (EDS.), NOCTURNAL MALAGASY PRIMATES: ECOLOGY, PHYSIOLOGY, AND BEHAVIOR, 1980 JAMES L. FOBES AND JAMES E. KING (EDS.), PRIMATE BEHAVIOR, 1982 DONALD E. KROODSMA AND EDWARD H. MILLER (EDS.), ACOUSTIC COMMUNICATION IN BIRDS, VOLUME 1: PRODUCTION, PERCEPTION, AND DESIGN FEATURES OF SOUNDS, 1982. VOLUME 2: SONG LEARNING AND ITS CONSEQUENCES, 1982 Acoustic Communication in Birds Volume 1 Production, Perception, and Design Features of Sounds Edited by DONALD E. KROODSMA Department of Zoology University of Massachusetts Amherst, Massachusetts EDWARD H. MILLER Vertebrate Zoology Division British Columbia Provincial Museum Victoria, British Columbia, Canada Taxonomic Editor HENRI OUELLET Vertebrate Zoology Division Museum of Natural Sciences National Museums Canada Ottawa, Ontario, Canada 1982 ACADEMIC PRESS A Subsidiary of Harcourt Brace Jovanovich, Publishers New York London Paris San Diego San Francisco Sao Paulo Sydney Tokyo Toronto COPYRIGHT © 1982, BY ACADEMIC PRESS, INC. ALL RIGHTS RESERVED. NO PART OF THIS PUBLICATION MAY BE REPRODUCED OR TRANSMITTED IN ANY FORM OR BY ANY MEANS, ELECTRONIC OR MECHANICAL, INCLUDING PHOTOCOPY, RECORDING, OR ANY INFORMATION STORAGE AND RETRIEVAL SYSTEM, WITHOUT PERMISSION IN WRITING FROM THE PUBLISHER. ACADEMIC PRESS, INC. 111 Fifth Avenue, New York, New York 10003 United Kingdom Edition published by ACADEMIC PRESS, INC. (LONDON) LTD. 24/28 Oval Road, London NW1 7DX Library of Congress Cataloging in Publication Data Main entry under title: Acoustic communication in birds. (Communication and behavior) Vol. 1: Edited by Donald E. Kroodsma, Edward H. Miller, and Henri Ouellet. Includes bibliographical references and indexes. 1. Bird-song. 2. Animal communication. I. Kroodsma, Donald E. II. Miller, Edward H. III. Ouellet, Henri. IV. Series. QL698.5.A26 598.2'59 82-6730 ISBN 0-12-426801-3 (v. 1) PRINTED IN THE UNITED STATES OF AMERICA 82 83 84 85 9 8 7 6 5 4 3 2 1 The contributors dedicate these volumes to Peter Marler. Peter's research and writings have placed the study of animal communication in the fore of modern evolutionary and ecological thought in ethology, and his impact is apparent from the sheer number and diversity of citations he receives in these two volumes. Research into almost any facet of bird acoustics puts one into contact with his work, on topics such as learning, geographic variation, species-specificity, "design features," individual variation, and grading. In addition, Peter has touched many workers indi­ vidually, as graduate students, postdoctoral re­ searchers, and peers, whereby his influence upon re­ search and thinking on animal communication has carried yet further. We are sure that this treatise reflects Peter Marler's influence upon the field of avian acoustics, and hope that it also reflects our esteem and respect for him as a scientist, teacher, and colleague. Contributors Numbers in parentheses indicate the pages on which the authors' contributions begin. Arthur P. Arnold (75), Department of Psychology, and Laboratory of Neuroen- drocrinology, Brain Research Institute, University of California, Los An­ geles, California 90024 Peter H. Becker (213), Institut fur Vogelforschung, Vogelwarte Helgoland, D 2940 Wilhelmshaven 15, West Germany John H. Brackenbury (53), Department of Biology, University of Salford, Sal- ford M5 4WT, England Clive K. Catchpole (297), Department of Zoology, Bedford College, University of London, London NW1 4NS, England Robert J. Dooling (95), Department of Psychology, University of Maryland, College Park, Maryland 20742 Edward H. Miller (253), Vertebrate Zoology Division, British Columbia Provin­ cial Museum, and Biology Department, University of Victoria, Victoria, British Columbia, Canada Eugene S. Morton (183), National Zoological Park, Smithsonian Institution, Washington, D.C. 20008 Douglas G. Richards (131), Kewalo Basin Marine Mammal Laboratory, Univer­ sity of Hawaii, Honolulu, Hawaii 96814 David C. Wickstrom (1), Library of Natural Sounds, Laboratory of Ornithology, Cornell University, Ithaca, New York 14850 R. Haven Wiley (131), Department of Zoology, University of North Carolina, Chapel Hill, North Carolina 27514 Foreword It is entirely appropriate that these volumes of collected essays on aspects of the study of bird vocalizations should be dedicated to Peter Marler. While the scien­ tific study of bird vocalizations was initiated by W. H. Thorpe, its subsequent expansion and elaboration was due primarily to Peter Marler. W. H. Thorpe was initially an entomologist. Finding that some aspects of insect behavior were more labile than had formerly been supposed, he came to feel that the most pressing problems in the study of animal behavior concerned the interface between "instinct," as it was then called, and learning. As an amateur ornithologist, he realized that birds provided exceptionally suitable ma­ terial for this work. Birds have a repertoire of relatively stereotyped movement patterns and yet a4t4 the same time exhibit marked learning ability. Accordingly, in 1950 he set up An Ornithological Field Station" (now called the Sub Depart­ ment of Animal Behaviour) at Madingley, Cambridge. Knowing that Chaffinch {Fringilla coelebs) song was subject to at least some individual variation and some flexibility (e.g., Poulsen, 1951), he decided to study the ontogeny of Chaffinch vocalization, and initiated a program of hand-rearing Chaffinches and studying their song development after varying exposure to the species' song (Thorpe, 1963). Peter Marler was the first graduate student at Madingley. When he came, he already had a Ph.D. in botany and a tenured job. It must have been a decision of considerable courage to give up that job in order to take a Ph.D. in a subject that really interested him. His Ph.D. was a field study of the Chaffinch: he wanted to work in the field and the choice of the Chaffinch as subject meshed well with Thorpe's own work. At that time I was studying the courtship of captive Chaf­ finches and I am sure that Peter will remember the long discussions, and some­ times disputes, that we had over whether this or that posture should be called the lopsided wings-drooped posture or something a bit snappier—issues that seemed terribly important to us then! Peter stayed on as a postdoctoral worker and took on the song learning work. His training as an all-round biologist stood him in good stead, and at that time he became interested not only in the ontogenetic problem that Thorpe was study- xiii xiv Foreword ing—how does Chaffinch song develop—but also in the other three questions with which ethologists are concerned—those of causation, function, and evolu­ tion. When he subsequently moved to Berkeley, he took the song problem with him. Changing his British speech for what seemed to some of us a rather extreme American accent, and the Chaffinch for White-crowned Sparrows (Zonotrichia leucophrys) and juncos, he started his well-known studies of ontogeny. At around that time the study of bird song really took off and it can now be regarded as having produced material of crucial importance for all four major questions of ethology. Taking the ontogenetic question first, three fundamentally important princi­ ples have been established largely or entirely through work on bird song. The principle of sensitive periods in development, though apparent from earlier work on imprinting and other phenomena, owes a great deal to studies of bird song. The fact that what an animal learns is constrained in part by its species, though again an idea coming also from other sources of evidence, was established most firmly in the 1950s and early 1960s by the work on bird song. And third, the view that the elaboration and perfection of the song pattern depend on compari­ son between the vocal output and a previously established template casts a new light on many aspects of ontogeny. In all of these issues Peter Marler and his colleagues played a leading role. Understanding the processes of song develop­ ment was facilitated by comparisons between studies of different species (e.g., Immelmann, 1969; Konishi, 1964; Nicolai, 1956), and Marler himself used a comparative approach to good effect, relating plasticity to the ecology of the species (Marler, 1967). Marler's field studies of the Chaffinch had inevitably alerted him to problems concerned with the causation of bird song, and his thesis contains a great deal of observational material on the factors determining when a Chaffinch sings. He soon became interested also in the patterning of bird song, and was one of the first people to study the detailed sequencing of different song types in an indi­ vidual's vocal output (e.g., Isaac and Marler, 1963). However, a more detailed study of the neural mechanisms underlying bird song arose from one of the findings of the studies of song ontogeny. Domestic Fowl (Gallus domes- ticus) and Ringed Turtle-Doves (Streptopelia risoria) were found to be capable of developing all the normal species' vocal signals, even though deafening took place soon after hatching. On the other hand, Konishi and Nottebohm (e.g., 1969) found that, in a number of species of songbirds, deafening before full song had developed resulted in consistent abnormalities in their song, and often in a regression to a rather amorphous type of vocal output. However, birds that had already learned to sing could continue singing after deafening. Since it was known that the control of human speech is influenced by the speaker's perception of himself speaking, the finding that deaf birds could continue to sing normally was surprising. One possibility was that the deafened bird was using feedback Foreword XV from the muscles. Nottebohm therefore investigated the effects of severing the hypoglossal nerves in the syrinx. This has led to the discovery of laterality in the motor control of bird vocalizations, and what is more of a partially reversible laterality. Furthermore, it has led to a detailed investigation of the brain mecha­ nisms underlying bird song, and has provided us with quite new data on the role of hormones in the development of neural mechanisms (e.g., Nottebohm, 1980). Marler's training as an all-round naturalist led him early on to ask functional questions. He was concerned with the functions of different calls in the Chaf­ finch's repertoire. What information did each carry? This led him into an attempt to categorize the nature of the information carried in animal signals (Marler, 1961). While not everyone will agree with the view that natural selection always acts to enhance the effectiveness of signals in transmitting information about the signaler, Marler's formulations greatly facilitated the study of communication. He was also concerned with the diversity of avian vocalizations. Noticing that the alarm calls of different species tended to resemble each other, while the songs were very different, he speculated about the selective factors controlling the form of avian vocalizations—selective factors that included the optimal degree of audibility in the habitat in question as well as the particular response to be elicited in the responding individual (e.g., Marler, 1955). In addition, in com­ paring the ontogeny of different avian species he was forced to ask why learning plays a much greater part in some species than others, why the sensitive period occurs at different ages in different species, why learning is constrained in one way in some species and in another way in others, and so on. We do not yet know the answers to all these questions, but it was Marler's pioneering work in the early 1950s that posed them. Finally, and inevitably, functional questions led to questions about the course of evolution of avian vocalizations. Peter Marler was concerned with this prob­ lem at a very early stage: as a student, he went on an expedition to the Canary Islands and became concerned with the differences between the songs of the species found there and their mainland counterparts (Marler and Boatman, 1951). His work on bird ontogeny led him into questions of the relations between song and call notes, and the functional questions he raised were inevitably linked with evolutionary questions about how song evolved. While Peter Marler's work on avian vocalizations has led to progress in answering all four of the major questions in which ethologists are interested, that is not all. Perhaps influenced by Thorpe, who was a pioneer in emphasizing the importance of perceptual processes at a time when most ethologists were think­ ing in relatively mechanistic terms and were not yet sensitive to his suggestions, Peter Marler became interested at an early stage in the relations between the physical structure of bird vocalization and their quality as perceived by the recipient (e.g., Marler, 1969). And while outside the scope of this volume, Marler's more recent work on primate vocalizations, and his emphasis on com- xvi Foreword mon features between avian communication and human language (Marler, 1970) has done much to stimulate research. I would suggest that the study of bird song is the example par excellence of the ethological approach. It involves the study of a naturally occurring pattern of behavior against a background of the natural history of the species concerned, but employs an experimental methodology. It involves questions about the causation, ontogeny, function, and evolution of the pattern in question, questions that are at the same time independent and interfertile. It is probably true to say that the study of bird song has done as much for the advancement of ethology as the study of any other specific aspect of behavior. In this, Peter Marler has played a major role. Robert A. Hinde REFERENCES Immelmann, K. (1969). Song development in the Zebra Finch and other estrilidid finches. In "Bird Vocalizations. Their Relation to Current Problems in Biology and Psychology" (R. A. Hinde, ed.), pp. 61-74. Cambridge Univ. Press, London and New York. Isaac, D., and Marler, P. (1963). Ordering of sequences of singing behaviour of Mistle Thrushes in relation to timing. Anim. Behav. 11, 179-188. Konishi, M. (1963). The role of auditory feedback in the vocal behavior of the Domestic Fowl. Z. Tierpsychol. 20, 349-367. Konishi, M. (1964). Effects of deafening on song development in two species of juncos. Condor 66, 85-102. Konishi, M., and Nottebohm, F. (1969). Experimental studies in the ontogeny of avian vocaliza­ tions. In "Bird Vocalizations. Their Relation to Current Problems in Biology and Psychol­ ogy" (R. A. Hinde, ed.), pp. 29-48. Cambridge Univ. Press, London and New York. Marler, P. (1955). Characteristics of some animal calls. Nature (London) 176, 6. Marler, P. (1961). The logical analysis of animal communication. J. Theoret. Biol. 1, 295-317. Marler, P. (1967). Comparative study of song development in sparrows. Proc. 14th Int. Ornith. Cong. Oxford, pp. 213-244, Blackwell, Oxford. Marler, P. (1969). Tonal quality of bird sounds. In "Bird Vocalizations. Their Relation to Current Problems in Biology and Psychology" (R. A. Hinde, ed.), pp. 5-18. Cambridge Univ. Press, London and New York. Marler, P. (1970). Birdsong and speech development: could there be parallels? Am. Sci. 58 (6), 669-673. Marler, P., and Boatman, D. J. (1951). Observations on the birds of Pico, Azores. Ibis 93, 90-99. Nicolai, J. (1956). Zur Biologie und Ethologie des Gimpels (Pyrrhula pyrrhula L.). Z. Tierpsychol. 13, 93-132. Nottebohm, F. (1980). Brain pathways for vocal learning in birds. A review of the first 10 years. Prog, in Psychobiol. and Physiol. Psychol. 9, 85-124. Poulsen, H. (1951). Inheritance and learning in the song of the Chaffinch (Fringilla coelebs). Behaviour 3, 216-228. Thorpe, W. H. (1963). "Learning and Instinct in Animals." Methuen, London. Preface We began corresponding about co-editing a book on bird sounds in 1978, excited by the enormous increase in evolutionary understanding and interpreta­ tion of communication systems since Robert Hinde's edited volume, Bird Vocal­ izations, appeared in 1969. The rapid mushrooming and splintering of ideas and observations on animal communication were daunting, but we nevertheless shared the belief that a representative and useful collection of writings on evolu­ tion and ecology of bird acoustics could be assembled. Our original intent was to compile both taxonomic and conceptual reviews, but the meager knowledge of acoustic signals of many important avian taxa made the first of these impossible. Consequently, we solicited contributions from active researchers in bird acous­ tics, for chosen areas of evolutionary and behavioral ecology, and sought to complement them with reviews of sound recording techniques, and sound pro­ duction, reception, and processing. Volume 1 begins with several of these background chapters. The first dis­ cusses sound recording, makes certain recommendations, and points out com­ mon errors. The others outline some of the complex events and processes be­ tween sound production and behavioral response to sound. The remaining chapters stand apart from the first ones, a gap that accurately reflects our poor understanding of the processes which ultimately link individual physiological responses to population-genetical changes, and to larger-scale evolutionary trends. Bridging this gap will require diverse research endeavors and theorizing, some of which are touched on in Chapters 5-9 of Volume 1 and Chapters 1-9 of Volume 2. The chapters are varied. They range from lengthy, well balanced, detailed syntheses of subjects such as coding of species-specificity (Becker), indi­ viduality (Falls), and environmental acoustics (Wiley and Richards), etc., to briefer more partisan explications of ideas and observations on dialects (Baker), subsong (Marler and Peters), sexual selection (Catchpole), "moti­ vation-structural rules" (Morton), etc. Others present novel views and reviews on doggedly troublesome concepts such as duetting (Farabaugh), vocal mimicry (Baylis), and geographic variation (Mundinger). We have contributed chapters xvii

See more

The list of books you might like

Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.