Cultural Studies of Science Education 11 Marilyn Fleer Niklas Pramling A Cultural- Historical Study of Children Learning Science Foregrounding Aff ective Imagination in Play-based Settings A Cultural-Historical Study of Children Learning Science Cultural Studies of Science Education Volume 11 Series Editors KENNETH TOBIN, City University of New York The Graduate Centre, New York, USA CATHERINE MILNE, Department of Teaching and Learning, New York University The Steinhardt School of Culture, Educat, New York, USA CHRISTINA SIRY, Faculty of Language and Literature, Huma, University of Luxembourg, Walferdange, Luxembourg MICHAEL P. MUELLER, Anchorage, Alaska, USA The series is unique in focusing on the publication of scholarly works that employ social and cultural perspectives as foundations for research and other scholarly activities in the three fi elds implied in its title: science education, education, and social studies of science. The aim of the series is to establish bridges to related fi elds, such as those concerned with the social studies of science, public understanding of science, science/technology and human values, or science and literacy. Cultural Studies of Science Education, the book series explicitly aims at establishing such bridges and at building new communities at the interface of currently distinct discourses. In this way, the current almost exclusive focus on science education on school learning would be expanded becoming instead a focus on science education as a cultural, cross-age, cross-class, and cross-disciplinary phenomenon. The book series is conceived as a parallel to the journal Cultural Studies of Science Education, opening up avenues for publishing works that do not fi t into the limited amount of space and topics that can be covered within the same text. More information about this series at http://www.springer.com/series/8286 Marilyn Fleer • Niklas Pramling A Cultural-Historical Study of Children Learning Science Foregrounding Affective Imagination in Play-b ased Settings Marilyn Fleer Niklas Pramling Monash University University of Gothenburg Frankston , VIC , Australia Gothenburg , Sweden ISSN 1879-7229 ISSN 1879-7237 (electronic) ISBN 978-94-017-9369-8 ISBN 978-94-017-9370-4 (eBook) DOI 10.1007/978-94-017-9370-4 Springer Dordrecht Heidelberg New York London Library of Congress Control Number: 2014947233 © Springer Science+Business Media Dordrecht 2015 T his work is subject to copyright. All rights are reserved by the Publisher, whether the whole or part of the material is concerned, specifi cally the rights of translation, reprinting, reuse of illustrations, recitation, broadcasting, reproduction on microfi lms or in any other physical way, and transmission or information storage and retrieval, electronic adaptation, computer software, or by similar or dissimilar methodology now known or hereafter developed. Exempted from this legal reservation are brief excerpts in connection with reviews or scholarly analysis or material supplied specifi cally for the purpose of being entered and executed on a computer system, for exclusive use by the purchaser of the work. Duplication of this publication or parts thereof is permitted only under the provisions of the Copyright Law of the Publisher’s location, in its current version, and permission for use must always be obtained from Springer. Permissions for use may be obtained through RightsLink at the Copyright Clearance Center. Violations are liable to prosecution under the respective Copyright Law. T he use of general descriptive names, registered names, trademarks, service marks, etc. in this publication does not imply, even in the absence of a specifi c statement, that such names are exempt from the relevant protective laws and regulations and therefore free for general use. While the advice and information in this book are believed to be true and accurate at the date of publication, neither the authors nor the editors nor the publisher can accept any legal responsibility for any errors or omissions that may be made. The publisher makes no warranty, express or implied, with respect to the material contained herein. Printed on acid-free paper Springer is part of Springer Science+Business Media (www.springer.com) Foreword Personally and as an educator, I have always found it helpful to think of Science as a human invention in which I could share. So it was pleasing to fi nd the authors of this book unequivocally describe Science as a human invention. Very often in school science, recognising or recording a natural phenomenon is confused with its sci- ence. Phrases like “discovery learning ” and “guided discovery” commonly confuse this recognition of a natural phenomenon with its subsequent description and expla- nation in Science. Making the distinction clear helps science educators to explore and teachers to employ pedagogical processes that move a learner from recognition to scientifi c understanding. This book is about a particular view of how early child- hood education can facilitate these processes. Describing Science as a great human invention locates it alongside those other great human inventions like Art, Music, God and Technology, each of which has an immanent (personal) sense and a transcendental (beyond the personal) sense. The cultural-historical foundation for early childhood science education that Marilyn Fleer and Niklas Pramling, the authors of this book, espouse matches these two senses, and they skilfully interrelate them. I n the late 1960s, soon after my career in science education began, I visited the Elementary Science Study (ESS) project in Massachusetts, a more interesting one of the many science curriculum projects then being developed on both sides of the Atlantic. A riveting comment by its director, Robert Hein, still comes easily to mind. “Science should be the easiest subject to teach in the early years of school- ing, because it only requires the abilities to see and to describe in talk what you are seeing, and the great majority of children entering school have these abilities already.” The materials for this project (and its counterpart in England, Nuffi eld Primary Science) suggested that teachers should encourage their young students to engage with a natural phenomenon and then develop the lesson from the ques- tions the students asked. There were many reasons why these projects failed, but three come to mind in relation to this book. I have already referred to one, namely these projects’ mantra of “discovery learn- ing” insuffi ciently recognised the invented nature of the science that described and explained these natural phenomena. The second is the assumption the projects made v vi Foreword that curiosity about nature is an innate quality we all share. I do not want here to be defi nitive about such a deep issue. We would not have Science at all if there had not been persons, since the dawn of humanity, who wondered in awe, and then in curi- osity about nature. It is enough to say that these responses were not equally shared when the ESS and Nuffi eld projects’ materials were tried in schools. Only some of the students were interested in the engagement phase and fewer still came up with investigable questions. Furthermore, many of the teachers in these early school years were not interested in or inspired by these phenomena. T his widespread lack of interest in the natural world is something I have had to become more and more aware of in the ensuing years. When I began to think about the issue of Science for All in the 1980s, I had to come to terms with the fact that most of my peers in school did not share the same fascination that I had with Science’s account of natural phenomena. Otherwise, they too would have chosen to study the sciences in senior secondary school and go on with them after school. So I can accept Fleer and Pramling’s premise that wonder in the face of nature is not innate but needs help to be acquired. This acquisition needs to be in both meanings of the word – the sense of sheer wonder (awe) of a phenomenon and the sense of wonder (starting to question) about the phenomenon. This book deals particularly with this second sense of wonder about the natural world (which is called curiosity) not being innately shared and importantly attends to how one common source, the experiences of pre-school education, can and should contribute to its learning. T he third reason relates to how rapidly formal teaching and learning about a natural phenomenon should move towards Science’s account of it. Too often, the school science curriculum has tried to rush this movement. I remember going into a bakery in a small country town in Victoria and being told by the baker that a local teacher had recently brought her prep year students (5-year-olds) on a visit. The baker told me he had been surprised that the focus of the visit was not on how bread and other products were made but on how any heavy objects he used were lifted and moved. Equally curious, I went to the school and talked to the teacher. She showed me a range of “heavy” objects that had been put in the playground for the students to explore how they might move them. Furthermore, she explained that her group of students had also visited the local garage and the small supermarket to see how their heavy objects were moved. Through these teaching experiences, the students had come up with a number of ways to move their set of objects. Each of these ways embodied in the students a set of experiences that would stand them in good stead in subsequent years at school to engage with the theoretical concepts and principles that Physics, as a science, has used to account for the movement of heavy material objects. A bout the same time, I came across the phrase “preconceptual learning” for the fi rst time. It was coined by the team in the New Zealand Children’s Science Project at the University of Waikato, which did so much in the 1980s to inform us all about the ideas and supporting rationales that pre-schoolers and young learners commonly hold about scientifi c phenomena and about the words like “force”, “fl oating and sinking”, “light and dark”, “life” and “hot and cold”. “Preconceptual learning” was, this team argued, a necessary step that had been very largely overlooked in school Foreword vii science. In other words, students were being expected to learn Lever Laws, Archimedes Principle, Laws of Refl ection and of Motion, the Characteristics of Living Things and Chemical Reactions before they had had any signifi cant experi- ence of the phenomena these scientifi c concepts and principles had been invented to describe and explain. This book reinforces the positive role the pre-school years can play in providing this rich base of experiential learning. I commend the authors for presenting so clearly the potential that the context of pre-school education has for the beginnings of science learning and commend it to teacher educators and their students – preservice teachers – to take up the opportuni- ties this book offers to them. Monash University , Clayton , VIC , Australia Peter Fensham Queensland University of Technology , Brisbane , QLD , Australia 16 August 2013 Pref ace In this book, we move beyond the traditional constructivist and social-constructivist view of learning and development in science. We argue that science as a body of knowledge is something that humans have constructed (historically) and recon- structed (contemporarily) to meet human needs. As such, this human invention acts as an evolving cultural tool for supporting and helping to understand everyday life. We draw upon cultural-historical theory in order to theorise early childhood science education in relation to our current globalised education contexts. We do not seek to make cultural comparisons, as are found in cross-cultural research. But, rather, we seek to better understand the many ways that science concepts are learned by very young children. T he book is designed for researchers and educators interested in a theoretical discussion of the cultural-historical foundation for early childhood science educa- tion. In a book of this kind, it is important to examine the contemporary theories of learning and development within the general fi eld of early childhood education. A theoretical examination of this kind allows for the foundational pedagogical con- text of the young learner to be interrogated. Through this kind of analysis, it is possible to examine play-based contexts in relation to opportunities for scientifi c conceptual development of young children. With this approach in mind, and with the empirical literature relevant to early childhood education examined, it is pos- sible to introduce a more relevant approach to the teaching of science and for the development of young children’s scientifi c thinking. In this book, we specifi cally present a pedagogical model for introducing scientifi c concepts to young children in play-based settings. ix