ebook img

Personality and Politics: Problems of Evidence, Inference, and Conceptualization PDF

246 Pages·1987·12.65 MB·English
Save to my drive
Quick download
Download
Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.

Preview Personality and Politics: Problems of Evidence, Inference, and Conceptualization

PERSONALITY AND POLITICS PERSONALITY AND POLITICS Problems of Evidence, Inference, and Conceptualization With a new preface FRED I. GREENSTEIN PRINCETON UNIVERSITY PRESS PRINCETON, NEW JERSEY To Amy, Jessica, and Michael Published by Princeton University Press, 41 William Street, Princeton, New Jersey 08540 In the United Kingdom Princeton University Press, Guildford, Surrey Preface to New Edition copyright © 1987 by Princeton University Presb All rights reserved Library of Congress Cataloging in Publication Data will be found on the last printed page of this book First printing, 1969 by Markham Publishing Company Norton edition, 1975 First Princeton Paperback printing, 1987 LCC 87-2427 ISBN 0-691-07731-2 ISBN 0-691-02260-7 (pbk ) Clothbound editions of Princeton University Press books are printed on acid- free paper, and binding materials are chosen for strength and durability Paperbacks, while satisfactory for personal collections, are not usually suitable for library rebinding Printed in the United States of America by Princeton University Press, Princeton, New Jersey Preface to New Edition This book addresses a ubiquitous real-world phenomenon and a con troversial scholarly specialization that bear the same name: personality and politics. Because I find it compellingly evident that in some con texts and at some times the phenomenon has been highly consequen tial, I consider it vital that paths be cleared through the tangle of intellectual underbrush that impedes more and better inquiry. The underbrush consists of disagreements among scholars about whether personality and politics warrants study and, if so, whether it is ame nable to disciplined study. Fundamental questions about how to ex plain human behavior in general and political behavior in particular underly these controversies. What is appropriate evidence? What kinds of inferences are plausible? What conceptual strategies are productive? I first encountered the controversies that were to be the "data" for this 1969 book in the 1950's, as I began to chart a personal path of inquiry into political psychology. I sought perspective on the debates in political science from other disciplines in which problems of linking psychological functioning to social phenomena also arise—for exam ple, sociology, psychology, anthropology, and history. I gained some clarity, but also encountered much the same debates. Indeed, debate seemed to be more extensive than research in some disciplines. In Chapter Two I quote the 1960 observation of sociologists David Ries- man and Nathan Glazer that the culture and personality literature (an analogue to the personality and politics literature) has "more critics than practitioners."1 There appears to be less contention now. But this is largely the result of compartmentalization in the social sciences. Increasingly, scholarly discourse takes place among specialists within narrow dis ciplinary areas rather than throughout or across disciplines. At the same time, the volume of work on political psychology, including issues 1 David Riesman and Nathan Glazer, "The Lonely Crowd: A Reconsideration in 1960," in Seymour M. Lipset and Leo Lowenthal, eds., Culture and Social Character (New York: The Free Press, 1961), p. 437. V VI PREFACE TO NEW EDITION related to personality and politics, has grown; and one kind of political psychology—the cognitive psychology of perception and mispercep- tion—has found a respected niche in a political science field, namely international relations.2 But consensus on the overall importance of such work has not substantially increased. The journal published by the International Society of Political Psychology—a group that was formed in the 1970's—has not become obligatory reading in the several disciplines from which the society draws members, and in the official journal of the American Political Science Association, articles ex­ plicitly addressed to personality and politics or political psychology are still just as rare as they were throughout the six decades from the appearance of the first issue of the journal to the publication of this book in 1969.3 The claims and counter-claims that I addressed in the 1960's on the merits of studying personality and politics still have not been resolved. A historian's 1985 review of a richly documented, closely argued psychobiography of Kemal Atatiirk, for example, taxed the 2 See for example, Robert Jervis, Perception and Misperception in International Politics (Princeton, N.J.: Princeton University Press, 1976); Richard Ned Lebow, Be­ tween Peace and War: The Nature of International Crisis (Baltimore: The Johns Hopkins University Press, 1981); Deborah W. Larson, Origins of Containment: Λ Psychological Explanation (Princeton, N.J.: Princeton University Press, 1985); and Robert Jervis, Richard Ned Lebow, and Janice Gross Stein, eds., Psychology and Deterrence (Baltimore: The Johns Hopkins University Press, 1985). For a collection of papers addressed generally to the role of cognition in political behavior, most of which report or discuss survey research on voting and public opinion, see Richard R. Lau and David 0. Sears, eds., Political Cognition (Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 1986). 1 A listing of key words in the titles of articles in the six decades of the American Political Science Review appears in Kenneth Janda, ed., Cumulative Index to the Amer­ ican Political Science Review, Volumes 1-62: 1906—1968 (Ann Arbor, Mich.: Xerox/ Universitv Microfilms, 1969) During this period the journal published only 17 articles that included the words "psychology" or "personality" in their titles. These references were dwarfed by references to governmental institutions and political processes: "Con­ gress" appeared in 140 titles, "political party" in 164, and so on Since 1968, references to "psychology" and "personality" have remained rare. It should be stressed that much psychological evidence is reported in American Political Science Review articles, es­ pecially in articles that have appeared since survey research became a major political science research procedure in the 1950's. Typically, when they refer to "personality," political scientists do not have in mind political attitudes and preferences of the sort reported in most survey research. Much of my analysis, however, is as applicable to attitudes and preferences as to any other kind of psychological disposition. In this I follow Henry A. Murray's usage, treating "personality" as "the most comprehensive term we have in psychology." Murray, "Personality: Contemporary Viewpoints: Com­ ponents of an Evolving Personological System," International Encyclopedia of the Social Sciences, 12 (New York: Macmillan and The Free Press, 1968), p. 9. PREFACE TO NEW EDITION Vll book's authors for employing psychological explanations of their pro tagonist. In the reviewer's opinion, a cultural explanation would have been more appropriate.4 This criticism is based on a fallacy I discuss in Chapter Two: that of treating sociocultural background as if it competes with psychology in explaining behavior. As is often true of disagreements about the study of personality and politics, the dispute about the relative merits of explaining behavior in terms of actors' backgrounds or their psychology can be constructively resolved. The former shape the latter; both, therefore, need to be studied as parts of causal chains. One of the polemical issues about the worth of studying personality and politics is of particular interest because it can be decided straight forwardly in a way that directly advances strategies of inquiry. I refer to the debate about whether actors' inner dispositions or their contem porary environments are the better predictors of their actions. The writings that emphasize the predictive power of psychology typically introduce qualifications to identify the kinds of situations in which individuals with different psychological proclivities are likely to behave the same. Similarly, writings that stress the environmental or, as it is often put, situational determinants of behavior commonly grant that some situations do not constrain actors to behave uniformly and thus permit differences in personal characteristics to affect behavior. Once each side's qualifications are noted, it becomes clear that there is substantial agreement among seeming disputants. In Chapter Two, I distill from the debate propositions about the circumstances under which political analysts will find it more or less necessary to take account of psychological dispositions and environments in order to explain political behavior. Moreover, an overarching insight emerges: it is essential for students of politics to heed Kurt Lewin's admonition that behavior must be analyzed in terms of the interaction of individual and situation.5 The most illuminating inquiries into political psychology do pre cisely this, identifying what in the terminology of research methods has come to be called interaction effects—relationships in which an 4 Kemal H. Karpat, "The Personality of Ataturk," American Historical Review, 90 (1985), 896. The book under review is Vamik D. Volkan and Norman Itzkowitz, The Immortal Ataturk: A Psychobwgraphy (Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1984). 5 Kurt Lewin, Principles of Topological Psychology (New York: McGraw-Hill, 1936), pp. 11-12. Vlll PREFACE TO NEW EDITION intervening variable must be identified in order to establish the effect of an independent variable on behavior. Thus, for example, Richard Christie and Florence Geis, in their meticulously designed study of individuals with personalities that predispose them to manipulate others (Machiavellianism), find consistent relationships between the disposition to be manipulative and action consistent with that dispo­ sition—but only under certain circumstances, such as in situations in which the potential for manipulation is clearly perceivable.6 In a parallel program of research into the psychology of leader­ ship, Fred Fiedler finds that environmental differences—notably, dif­ ferent requirements for effectiveness in various roles—are key inter­ vening variables. Role A will elicit effective performance by personality type X, while role Β suits personality type Y. In effect echoing Gordon Allport's aphorism that "the same heat that melts the butter hardens the egg,"7 Fiedler notes that specific leadership traits do not in themselves predict leaders' performance. Because "one can be an effective leader in some situations and not in others . . . a single leadership trait or combination of traits will not enable prediction in all situations." Thus leadership needs to be studied in terms of the interaction of person and situation, since "a trait or combination of traits may well predict performance in certain situations that can be specified. . . ."8 Alexander and Juliette George provide a perfect il­ lustration in their observation (discussed in Chapter Three) that Wood- row Wilson's intense need for power made him highly flexible when he was seeking to gain power but obdurately rigid when he was seeking to maintain it. In the research of Christie and Geis and of Fiedler, environmental variables intervene between psychological differences and behavior. On the other hand, in his psycho-physiological studies of the anxiety levels of soldiers in Vietnam combat, Peter Bourne finds that differ­ ences in environmental context (roles) produce different intervening 6 Richard Christie and Florence L. Geis, Studies in Machiavellianism (New York: Academic Press, 1970). 7 Gordon W. Allport, Personality: Λ Psychological Interpretation (New York: Holt, 1937), p. 325. 8 Fred E. Fiedler, Leadership (New York General Learning Press, 1971), p. 15. See also his "What Triggers the Person-Situation Interaction in Leadership," in David Magnusson and Norman S. Endler, eds-, Personality at the Crossroads: Current Issues in Interactional Psychology (Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 1977), pp. 151—63 and the sources there cited. PREFACE TO NEW EDITION IX psychological states and therefore may have different consequences for the behavior of individuals who do not vary systematically in un­ derlying psychological makeup. Bourne was able to interview soldiers at times when they were highly vulnerable to Viet Cong attack and to obtain a medical measure of the adrenal levels in their body. Some faced combat with anxious apprehension, others with fatalism, if not equanimity. All of the officers fell into the first category and most of the enlisted men into the second. But the differences in anxiety level did not come from character traits that differentiated officers from enlisted personnel. Rather, the officers were directly aware of how tenuous their circumstances were, the bulk of enlisted men were not. Among the enlisted men, however, the radio operators had the same information as the officers and were just as anxious.9 A turning point in research on personality occurred in 1975, when the first international symposium was convened on "person by situation interaction" as a basic perspective for the analysis of behavior. The resulting volume on "interactional psychology" and subsequent work under the same heading provides an invaluable body of basic research, conceptualization, and methodological innovation for applied study of behavior in particular institutional arenas such as politics.10 An interactional perspective sensitizes the political analyst to assess the effects on behavior not only of psychological predispositions and environmental contexts but also of different kinds of psychological predispositions. There is a tradition in political psychology, going back at least to Harold Lasswell's 1930 Psychopathology and Politics, of being especially attentive to irrational political behavior that is deeply rooted in the emotions and is most readily explicable in terms of the psy- chodynamic theories of neurosis and ego defense that stem from the work of Freud.11 In Chapter Four, I illustrate the problems of con- 9 Peter G. Bourne, "Altered Adrenal Function in Two Combat Situations in Viet Nam," in Β. E. Elefthenou and J. P. Scott, eds., The Physiology of Aggression and Defeat (New York: Plenum, 1971), pp. 265-305. Bourne's work does not assess the effect of emotional excitation on behavior. The extensive research on this topic is instructively discussed in Irving L. Janis and Leon Mann, Decision Making: A Psycho­ logical Analysis of Conflict, Choice and Commitment (New York: The Free Press, 1977). 10 Magnusson and Endler, eds., Personality at the Crossroads. For further contri­ butions to this literature, see Lawrence A. Pervin and Michael Lewis, eds.. Perspectives in Interactional Psychology (New York: Plenum, 1978). " Harold D. Lasswell, Psychopathology and Politics (1930; rpt. Chicago: Univer­ sity of Chicago Press, 1977).

See more

The list of books you might like

Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.