ebook img

ERIC EJ832373: Teaching Empathy Skills to Children with Autism PDF

2009·0.31 MB·English
by  ERIC
Save to my drive
Quick download
Download
Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.

Preview ERIC EJ832373: Teaching Empathy Skills to Children with Autism

JOURNALOFAPPLIEDBEHAVIORANALYSIS 2009, 42, 17–32 NUMBER1 (SPRING2009) TEACHING EMPATHY SKILLS TO CHILDREN WITH AUTISM JESSICA A. SCHRANDT QUEENSCOLLEGEANDTHEGRADUATECENTEROFTHECITY UNIVERSITYOFNEWYORK DAWN BUFFINGTON TOWNSEND INSTITUTEFOREDUCATIONALACHIEVEMENT AND CLAIRE L. POULSON QUEENSCOLLEGEANDTHEGRADUATECENTEROFTHECITY UNIVERSITYOFNEWYORK The purpose of this study was to teach empathetic responding to 4 children with autism. Instructorspresentedvignetteswithdollsandpuppetsdemonstratingvarioustypesofaffectand used prompt delay, modeling, manual prompts, behavioral rehearsals, and reinforcement to teach participants to perform empathy responses. Increases in empathetic responding occurred systematicallywiththeintroductionoftreatmentacrossallparticipantsandresponsecategories. Furthermore, responding generalized from training to nontraining probe stimuli for all participants.Generalizationoccurredfromdollsandpuppetstoactualpeopleinanontraining setting for 2 participants. Generalization was observed initially to the nontraining people and setting for the other participants, but responding subsequently decreased to baseline levels. Introduction oftreatment inthis setting produced rapidacquisition of targetskills. DESCRIPTORS: autism,empathy, socialskills _______________________________________________________________________________ Deficits in empathy and perspective taking states, take the perspective of another person, have been well documented in children with and respond with empathy. autism (e.g., Rutter, 1978; Sigman, Kasari, Empathy and perspective taking serve an Kwon, & Yirmiya, 1992; Yirmiya, Sigman, important role in what Rheingold and Hay Kasari,&Mundy,1992).Forexample,Yirmiya (1980) called prosocial behavior (e.g., helping, et al. demonstrated that after watching video sharing, turn taking). Eisenberg (1992) and segments of children experiencing different Rheingold, Hay, and West (1976) observed events and emotions, high-functioning children prosocial behavior, such as sharing and giving with autism were less able than their typically comfort, in typically developing children as developing peers to label others’ emotional young as 18 months old. Typically developing childrendisplayperspective-takingskillsaround 4 years of age (MacNamara, Baker, & Olson, 1976; Wimmer & Perner, 1983). It is easy to We thank Alana Bellizzi, Casey Gaetano, Julia Smith, see why such behavior is important in the and Jennifer Williams (teachers at the Institute for Educational Achievement), and Paul Argott, all of whom development of social relationships. Peers and assisted in carrying out this study. Special thanks to the family members may initiate social interaction participants and their families for making this study more frequently with children who take the possible. Correspondence concerning this article should be perspective of and demonstrate empathy to- addressed to Jessica Schrandt, 4048 Harriet Ave., wards others. Minneapolis, Minnesota 55409 (e-mail: jessyschrandt@ Althoughmanyresearchershavedocumented yahoo.com). doi:10.1901/jaba.2009.42-17 deficits in empathy andperspective-taking skills 17 18 JESSICA A. SCHRANDT et al. in individuals with autism (e.g., Baron-Cohen, limited number of cues when presented with Leslie, & Frith, 1985; Bauminger & Kasari, complex stimuli (Bailey, 1981; Lovaas, Koegel, 1999; Sigman et al., 1992; Yirmiya et al., & Schreibman, 1979; Schreibman, Koegel, & 1992), fewer have focused on the extent to Craig, 1977). When learning new discrimina- which these and related social skills can be tions,individualswithautismmayattendtofew taught. In the studies reviewed, modeling, or irrelevant stimuli in the environment. Thus, prompting, and reinforcement were the most failures in empathy (or other complex social frequently used and effective procedures for behavior) may not reflect a deficit of necessary teaching social skills. responses in the repertoire but rather an Gena, Krantz, McClannahan, and Poulson inability to differentiate the stimuli in the (1996) used modeling, prompting, and rein- presence of which specific responses (e.g., forcement to teach 4 children with autism offering assistance, demonstrating interest) contextually appropriate affective responding would be appropriate. to various scenarios (e.g., showing sympathy, In the natural environment, discriminative laughing about absurdities). The treatment stimuli for empathetic responding are both resulted in increased appropriate affect across varied and complex. In previous studies, participants and response categories, and these researchers effectively promoted generalization skills generalized across nontraining scenarios, of social skills from training to nontraining therapists, time, and settings. stimuli by (a) training with multiple exemplars Similarly, Harris, Handleman, and Alessan- within a stimulus class (e.g., showing a variety dri (1990) used prompting and reinforcement of reasons for sadness), and (b) training with to teach 3 adolescent boys to display helping discriminative stimulus compounds (e.g., pre- responses in a variety of situations. Although senting a combination of vocal and nonvocal responding generalized from training to non- ‘‘sadness’’ stimuli). training people and settings to some extent for In the present study, we sought to examine each of the boys, levels of generalized respond- whether procedures used previously to teach ing were lower than for training responding. other social skills could be extended to teaching Expanding on the Harris et al. (1990) study, empathy skills to children with autism. Specif- Reeve, Reeve, Townsend, and Poulson (2007) ically, the purpose of the present study was to used video modeling, prompting, and rein- assess the extent to which a treatment package forcement to teach 4 children with autism to consisting of the presentation of affective emit helping responses in the presence of discriminative stimulus compounds, prompt multiple discriminative stimuli from a variety delay, modeling through auditory scripts, of helping categories (e.g., replacing broken manual prompts, behavioral rehearsals, and materials, locating objects, putting away items). reinforcementwaseffectiveinteachingempathy Thetreatment packagewas effectivein teaching skills in a pretend-play setting to children with helping responses during training trials, and autism. Behavioral rehearsals and prompt delay responding generalized across nontraining stim- (Charlop, Shreibman, & Thibodeau, 1985; uli and response categories. Halle, Marshall, & Spradlin, 1979) were used The aforementioned studies, like the present to prevent and correct errors, which were study,approacheddeficitsinsocialbehaviorasa judged to be a potential difficulty. We also stimulus control problem. Many behavioral assessed the extent to which empathy skills deficits in individuals with autism have been generalized from training to nontraining probe attributed to the phenomenon of stimulus stimuli and from training dolls and puppets to overselectivity, the tendency to respond to a actual people in a nontraining setting. TEACHING EMPATHY SKILLS 19 METHOD mostly of single words and short phrases, and hissocialinitiationswereusuallynonvocal(e.g., Participants smiling, walking toward a person). Like Jacob, Participantswere4childrenwithautismwho he did not demonstrate empathy responses to attended an education program offering center- displays of affect by others. based and in-home behavioral intervention for Ali was 8 years 9 months old at the start of children with autism for 5.5 hr per day, 5 days the study. She obtained a standard score of 40 perweek.Theywereidentifiedforparticipation (age equivalent: 3 years 0 months) on the based on anecdotal reports from their teachers PPVT-III and 41 (age equivalent: 3 years 11 that they did not regularly demonstrate empa- months) on the EVT. Her language consisted thy toward others. All participants engaged in mostly of single words and short phrases. She minimal stereotypic or disruptive behavior and rarely demonstrated empathy skills, although had experience with token motivational sys- she sometimes said ‘‘sorry’’ when she acciden- tems. In addition, all participants had prereq- tally bumped into another person. uisite skills in vocal imitation of three-word phrases modeled by an instructor and on Setting and Materials auditory recordings. Sessions were conducted in a small room Josh was 4 years 5 months old at the start of furnished with small tables, chairs, and book- the study. He obtained a standard score of shelves. Toys were stored on the shelves, and 114 (age equivalent: 5 years 7 months) on the dolls and puppets used for training and probe Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test (3rd ed., trialswereplacedinseparatebinsinfrontofthe PPVT-III) and 106 (age equivalent: 4 years 10 shelves. A Language Master was placed on one months) on the Expressive Vocabulary Test of the tables. A digital video camera was placed (EVT). In addition to the behavioral interven- in one corner of the room and was used to tion program, he also attended an integrated record some sessions. preschool classroom for several hours per day, spoke in full sentences with clear articulation, Vignettes and Target Responses and established eye contact regularly during Affective discriminative stimulus compounds socialinteraction.Althoughhesometimesmade containedonemotorandonevocalcomponent. comments when observing displays of affect by They were presented in brief vignettes in which others (e.g., ‘‘crying,’’ ‘‘sad’’), he rarely dis- an instructor held up a doll or puppet and played appropriate empathy responses. pretended to make it say a statement paired Jacobwas6yearsoldatthestartofthestudy. with an action. Vignettes fell into three He obtained a standard score of 61 (age categories: sadness or pain, happiness or equivalent: 2 years 9 months) on the PPVT- excitement, and frustration. For example, in IIIand41(ageequivalent:2years4months)on the sadness or pain category, a vignette the EVT. His language consisted mostly of consisted of the instructor holding a puppet, requests, he rarely initiated social interaction, making it bump the table, and saying ‘‘ouch!’’ andhiseyecontactandattendingtootherswere as thoughthepuppet hadhurt itsleg.Vignettes often poor. He demonstrated no empathy were assigned randomly to training and probe responses to displays of affect by others. conditions. Sets of training and probe dolls and Luke was 5 years 6 months old at the start of puppets were matched based on similarity (e.g., the study. He obtained a standard score of 56 each set contained boys, girls, and animals). To (age equivalent: 1 year 10 months) on the promote generalization from training to non- PPVT-III and 55 (age equivalent: 2 years 7 training stimuli, dolls and puppets were not months) on the EVT. His language consisted assigned to specific vignettes. 20 JESSICA A. SCHRANDT et al. Table 1 Training andProbeDiscriminative Stimuli andTraining Responses for the Sadness or Pain Category Discriminativestimuli Responses Training ‘‘Ouch!’’andbumpinglegontable ‘‘Areyouokay?’’andpattingarm ‘‘Idon’tfeelgood’’andsittingdown ‘‘Areyoualright?’’andpattingarm ‘‘Ihurtmyelbow’’andtouchingelbow ‘‘It’sokay’’andpattingarm ‘‘Mystomachhurts’’andrubbingstomach ‘‘Oww!’’andfallingdown ‘‘I’msoupset’’andsnifflingorcrying ‘‘Iburnedmyhand!’’andgrabbingtoastanddroppingit Generalization ‘‘Ohno!’’andwipingeyeswithtissue ‘‘Icutmyfinger!’’andcuttingwithscissors ‘‘Ihaveaheadache’’andholdingforehead Empathy was operationally defined as a doll or puppet. The prompter, seated behind contextually appropriate response to a display the participant, waited for a designated interval of affect by a doll, puppet, or person that foraresponseandthendeliveredaconsequence contained motor and vocal components (in any (following training trials) or gave no feedback order) and began within 3 s of the end of the (following baseline and probe trials). A baseline display. For the previous example, the partici- orprobetrialendedwhen3 selapsed.Although pant could be taught to say, ‘‘Are you okay?’’ 3 of the participants received training in only and to pat the puppet’s arm. One motor the sadness or pain response category, baseline response and three different vocal responses trialsfortheothertwocategorieswerepresented were taught for each response category. To during each session. Dolls, puppets, toys, and a promotegeneralitytonontrainingvignettes,the token motivational system were placed on the responses were designed such that any response tablebetweentheparticipantandtheinstructor. taught for a particular vignette was appropriate Priortoeachsession,participantschosefroman for any vignette in the same response category. arrayofpreferredsnacksandactivitiesthatwere In addition, other contextually appropriate delivered in exchange for every 10 tokens responses not targeted during training were earned throughout the session (approximately scoredascorrect.Forexample,inthesadnessor three times per session). pain category, saying, ‘‘That’s too bad’’ while Baseline. Throughout the session, the giving the doll a hug was scored correct even prompter delivered one token per trial for though this response was not targeted directly. appropriate sitting and attending to the vi- The same response was not, however, scored gnettes. Tokens were delivered randomly either correct for the happiness or excitement catego- between trials as the instructor set up materials ry, because it was not contextually appropriate. for the next vignette or directly before the Acompletelistofvignettesandtargetresponses instructor presented the vignette. Instructors for each response category is provided in and prompters did not deliver explicit instruc- Tables 1, 2, and 3. tions or prompts to the participant regarding the dolls, puppets, or toys on the table. Correct Procedure responses were followed by a conversational General procedure. Sessions were conducted exchange by the instructor (e.g., ‘‘thank you’’). four to five times per week and lasted No feedback was given for incorrect responses. approximately 20 to 30 min. A trial began Treatment package for training sessions. Train- when the instructor presented a vignette with a ingsessionsconsistedof30trials(seventraining TEACHING EMPATHY SKILLS 21 Table 2 Training andProbe Discriminative Stimuli andTraining Responses for the Happiness or ExcitementCategory Discriminativestimuli Responses Training ‘‘Thisisfun!’’andpushingcar ‘‘CanIsee?’’andmovinghandtowardownbody ‘‘Ifinished!’’andcoloringpicture ‘‘Showme!’’andmovinghandtowardownbody ‘‘Ididit!’’andcompletingpuzzle ‘‘Letmesee!’’andmovinghandtowardownbody ‘‘Watchme!’’andstretchingSlinky ‘‘Lookatthis!’’andshowinggiftbox ‘‘Iwon!’’andplayingHungryHungryHipposandputtingboth handsintheair ‘‘Ifoundsomething!’’andshowinglizard Generalization ‘‘Ilovethis!’’androllingAnimaniacsball ‘‘LookwhatIgot!’’andshowingMagicWheel ‘‘Thisisthebest!’’andplayingfrogpiano and three nontraining probe-stimulus trials per prompter decreased the delay by 1 s if the response category) presented in random order. participant responded correctly on fewer than The prompter delivered manual and auditory fouroftheseventrainingtrials.Duringtraining prompts according to a prompt-delay sequence trials, the instructor delivered tokens and (Charlop et al., 1985; Halle et al., 1979) to behavior-specific praise following all correct prevent errors. Specifically, the prompter used unprompted responses. manual and auditory prompts immediately (0-s A behavioral rehearsal sequence was used for delay) for three sessions when training was all training trials in the 0-s delay condition and introduced for a particular response category. when the participant did not respond or During the fourth session, the prompter used a responded incorrectly in all other prompt-delay 1-s delay. The prompter increased the delay conditions. First, the prompter manually length by 1 s with a maximum of a 3-s delay prompted a correct motor response and simul- following each session in which the participant taneously played an auditory script on a respondedcorrectlyforatleastfouroftheseven Language Master, a device that plays aloud trials in a particular response category. The phrases recorded on a strip of magnetic tape Table 3 Training andProbe Discriminative StimuliandTraining Responses for the Frustration Category Discriminativestimuli Responses Training ‘‘Thiswon’twork!’’andputtingSylvestercarontable ‘‘Icanhelpyou’’andreachingonehandtowardpuppet/person,palm turnedslightlyup ‘‘Ican’tdoit!’’andtryingtoputshapeinsorter ‘‘Wantsomehelp?’’andreachingonehandtowardpuppetorperson, palmturnedslightlyup ‘‘Ican’tgetit’’andtryingtozipupcoat ‘‘I’llhelpyou’’andreachingonehandtowardpuppetorperson,palm turnedslightlyup ‘‘Ohman!’’anddroppingbinofblocks ‘‘It’stoohard!’’andtryingtoputK’Nexpiecestogether ‘‘It’sstuck!’’andtryingtoremovelidfrombox ‘‘Ibrokeit!’’andholdingupbrokenLegomodel Generalization ‘‘It’snotworking!’’andtryingtocolorwithmarkerwithcapon ‘‘Myarmisstuck!’’andtryingtoputonsweater ‘‘Thisistootight!’’andtryingtoopenjar 22 JESSICA A. SCHRANDT et al. along the bottom of a card that is pushed Experimental Design throughit.Iftheparticipantdidnotimitatethe A multiplebaselinedesign acrossparticipants auditory script, the prompter played the card was used to assess the extent to which the again and waited for a response. If the treatment package was effective in teaching participant did not imitate the auditory script empathetic responding to displays of sadness or after two presentations, the prompter provided pain to 4 participants (Josh, Jacob, Luke, and vocal prompts (e.g., ‘‘Say ‘It’s okay’’’) until the Ali).For1participant(Josh),asecondmultiple participant correctly imitated the script. Then, baseline design across response categories was the prompter played the auditory script again used to assess the extent to which the treatment and repeated this sequence until the participant package was effective in teaching empathetic correctly imitated the auditory script immedi- responding across three response categories ately after it was played on the Language (sadness or pain, happiness or excitement, and Master. Once the participant imitated the frustration). auditory script without prompts, the instructor Data Collection and Interobserver Agreement presented the same vignette, and the prompter Theexperimenterandoneoffourinstructors partially prompted the vocal and motor re- conducted interobserver agreement assessments sponses (e.g., played the first word of the for at least 35% of sessions in each of the auditory script and lightly tapped the partici- experimental conditions for each participant. pant’s arm). If the participant responded Observers met a criterion of 80% agreement in correctly, the prompter then gave the partici- data collection before agreement measures pant an opportunity to respond to the same began. Independent observers sat at separate vignettewithoutprompts. If the participantdid tables and recorded motor and vocal responses not respond or an error occurred, the sequence verbatim. For each trial, they circled ‘‘yes’’ for was repeated until the participant emitted a responses that contained both a motor and correct independent response. vocal component, were contextually appropri- Generalization to nontraining probe stimuli. ate, and began within 3 s of the end of the These trials were randomly interspersed vignette, and ‘‘no’’ for responses that did not throughout all training sessions (including meet these requirements. Agreements were baseline sessions); consisted of vignettes similar defined as trials in which both observers circled to those used during training trials; and ‘‘yes’’ or ‘‘no,’’ and disagreements were defined incorporated dolls, puppets, and toys never astrialsinwhichoneobservercircled‘‘yes’’and associated with training. During these trials, the other circled ‘‘no.’’ Agreements were instructors and prompters followed the same divided by agreements plus disagreements, and procedures used during the baseline condition. this ratio was converted to a percentage. Across Generalization to nontraining people and all participants and experimental conditions, setting. These sessions occurred approximately interobserver agreement ranged from 83% to once per week during baseline and training. 100%, indicating no more than one disagree- Sessions consisted of seven trials per response ment per response category in a single session. category. An actual person replaced the dolls and puppets and presented the training vi- Procedural Integrity gnettes used in the training setting. Sessions To ensure the integrity of the independent were conducted in a large conference room not variable, procedural integrity measures were associated with training. Again, instructors and collected during all interobserver agreement prompters followed the same procedures used sessions. Independent observers assessed accu- during the baseline condition. rate presentation of auditory and manual TEACHING EMPATHY SKILLS 23 prompts as well as accurate delivery of tokens presented for the happiness or excitement and for each trial during all conditions. Prompt frustrationresponsecategories.Becausevirtually delivery was considered accurate if prompts no responding occurred during baseline, all weredeliveredonlyafterthedesignatedamount baseline segments showed participants respond- of time during the prompt-delay condition or ing incorrectly or not at all. All segments from when participants responded incorrectly during treatment sessions showed responses that had a training trial. Token delivery was considered been scored as correct. Observers were given a accurate if it occurred only following attending written questionnaire and for each segment behavior unrelated to the training task (e.g., circled‘‘yes’’or‘‘no’’forthequestion,‘‘Didthe propersitting)duringbaselineorprobetrialsor child demonstrate empathy toward the doll or following a correct unprompted response dur- puppet?’’ ing training trials. For each trial, observers circled ‘‘yes’’ if RESULTS promptsandtokensweredeliveredaccuratelyor ‘‘no’’ if prompts and tokens were not delivered Figures 1 (Jacob, Luke, Ali) and 2 (Josh) accurately. Agreements were defined as trials in depict the number of motor and vocal empathy whichbothobserverscircled‘‘yes’’or‘‘no,’’and responses emitted to displays of affect with disagreements were defined as trials in which training stimuli. During the baseline condition, one observer circled ‘‘yes’’ and the other circled all participants responded infrequently to ‘‘no.’’ Agreements were divided by agreements displays of affect by the dolls and puppets. plusdisagreements,andthisratiowasconverted Increased pretend-play empathetic responding to a percentage. The mean percentage of occurredsystematicallywiththeintroductionof accurate presentation of prompts and tokens treatment across participants. was 99.9% across all participants and condi- Figures 3 (Jacob, Luke, Ali) and 4 (Josh) tions (range, 98% to 100%). Mean interob- depict the number of motor and vocal empathy server agreement on accurate presentation of responses emitted to displays of affect with prompts and tokens was 99.9% (range, 98% to nontraining probe stimuli. Appropriate re- 100%). sponding generalized from training to non- training vignettes, dolls, and puppets for all Social Validity participants, increasing systematically with the Nineteen teachers from the behavioral inter- introduction of treatment with training stimuli vention program who were not involved in the across participants. For Josh and Jacob, gener- study assessed the social validity of the results. alized motor and vocal responses emerged at Observers watched videotaped segments of about the same time. Luke’s generalized motor each of the participants responding to various and vocal responding with nontraining probe vignettes with dolls and puppets. Each segment stimuli was variable, with vocal responding (approximately 10 to 15 s in duration) showed emerging more quickly than motor responding. the instructor presenting a vignette with a doll For Ali, the opposite was true; generalized or puppet and the participant’s response, but motorrespondingemergedslightlymorequick- did not depict instructor feedback. For all ly than generalized vocal responding. participants, three baseline and three treatment Figures 5 (Jacob, Luke, Ali) and 6 (Josh) segments were selected randomly from the last depict the number of motor and vocal empathy three baseline and treatment sessions for the responses to displays of affect by Person 1 and sadness or pain response category and were Person 2 in a nontraining setting. For Josh and presented in random order. For Josh, three Jacob, motor and vocal responding generalized baselineandthree treatmentsegmentswere also from training dolls and puppets to a teacher 24 JESSICA A. SCHRANDT et al. Figure1. Numberofmotor(filledcircles)andvocal(opencircles)empathyresponsestodisplaysofsadnessorpain withtraining stimulifor Jacob, Luke, andAli. (Person 1) in a nontraining classroom. In Person1systematicallywiththeintroductionof addition, both boys responded to displays of treatment with training stimuli in the training affect by another teacher (Person 2) in the setting, although motor responding during this nontraining setting, although no baseline mea- condition returned to baseline levels during the surements were taken with this person (aside last two probe sessions. With the brief intro- from a single observation in Session 25 in the duction of training (i.e., vignettes presented by frustration category for Josh). Like Josh and Person 1, 3-s prompt delay, auditory prompts, Jacob, Luke and Ali demonstrated generalized manual prompts, behavioral rehearsals, tokens motor and vocal empathetic responding with delivered for correct responses) in the nontrain- TEACHING EMPATHY SKILLS 25 Figure 2. Number of motor (filled circles) and vocal (open circles) empathy responses to displays of affect with training stimulifor Josh. ing setting (Sessions 55 and 56), motor to Person 2 were also observed for both Luke responding increased to the same high level as and Ali, although, as with Person 1, vocal vocal responding for both Luke and Ali. responding was emitted at higher levels than Increases in generalized empathetic responding motor responding. 26 JESSICA A. SCHRANDT et al. Figure3. Numberofmotor(filledcircles)andvocal(opencircles)empathyresponsestodisplaysofsadnessorpain withnontraining probestimuli for Jacob, Luke,andAli.

See more

The list of books you might like

Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.