ebook img

ERIC ED603912: Classwide Extensions of Vocabulary Intervention Improve Learning of Academic Vocabulary by Preschoolers PDF

0.85 MB·English
by  ERIC
Save to my drive
Quick download
Download
Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.

Preview ERIC ED603912: Classwide Extensions of Vocabulary Intervention Improve Learning of Academic Vocabulary by Preschoolers

JSLHR Research Article Classwide Extensions of Vocabulary Intervention Improve Learning of Academic Vocabulary by Preschoolers Yagmur Seven,a Katharine Hull,a Keri Madsen,a John Ferron,a Lindsey Peters-Sanders,a Xigrid Soto,a Elizabeth S. Kelley,b and Howard Goldsteina Purpose:Manypreschoolers,especiallythosefromlow- inconsistentorminimaleffectsweredemonstratedby incomehouseholds,wouldbenefitfrominstructionto 8participants.Multilevelmodelingusedtoevaluatethe enrichtheirvocabularyandlanguagerepertoires.Yet, effectsstatisticallyrevealedstrongtreatmenteffects.In explicitinstructionofvocabularyandlanguageskills addition,the10childrenwithabove-averagelanguage generallyoccursinfrequentlyinearlychildhoodeducation showedimpressivelearningofvocabularywordsfrom settings.Thisstudyinvestigatedtheadditiveeffectsof bookssubjecttoteacherreviewstrategiesincomparison teacher-led,classwidereviewstrategiestoapreviously towordsfrombookstowhichtheywerenotexposed. studiedsmall-groupinterventiononchildren’slearningof Teachersvariedintheextenttowhichtheyimplemented academicvocabulary. reviewstrategiesintheirclassrooms.Nevertheless,their Method:Participantsincluded23childrenwithlimitedoral responsestosocialvalidityassessmentswerepositive, languageskillsatriskforreadingdifficultiesenrolledin supportingthefeasibilityofthisintervention. single-caseexperimentaldesigns.Effectsoftheclassroom Conclusions:Theadditionofclasswidereviewand strategiesalonealsowereexaminedin10childrenwith practiceopportunitiesisaneffectivemeansof above-averagelanguageabilitiesfrom2classrooms. enhancingtheeffectsofaneasy-to-implementsmall- Results:Visualanalysesoftheadaptedalternating groupinterventionthatteacheschallengingvocabulary treatmentsdesignsshowedconsistentlearning wordswithinprerecordedstories.Thisapproachholds improvementswhenvocabularyinstructionwasextended promiseasawaytoshrinkthepervasivewordgapthat intotheclassroomfor12children,ceilingeffectswere typicallyexistswhenchildreninhigh-povertycommunities evidentfor3participantsregardlessofcondition,and enterschool. D evelopmentoforallanguageiscriticalduring schoolwithlimitedvocabularyarepronetodeveloping earlychildhood,providingafoundationforliter- literacydifficultiesthat persist throughout school(Hart acyandnearlyallotheracademicperformance &Risley,1995;Hoff,2013;NationalResearchCouncil, (Roth, Speece, & Cooper, 2002; Walker, Greenwood, 1998).Unfortunately,childrenexhibitwidedifferencesin Hart,&Carta,1994). Specifically,orallanguageisknown vocabularyknowledgeuponschoolentry,placingmany tobeareliablepredictorofreadingproficiencywithaclear children at risk for academic disadvantage (National relationemergingbetweenearlyvocabularyknowledgeand ResearchCouncil,1998; Walker et al., 1994). Because laterreadingcomprehension(Kendeou,vandenBroek, the stakes are high for children with limited vocabulary White,&Lynch,2009;Quinn,Wagner,Petscher,&Lopez, knowledge,effortstoimproveorallanguageandvocabu- 2015;Rothetal.,2002).Theconsequencesofpoorlydevel- laryinat-riskpreschoolchildrenareparamount. opedvocabularycanbedevastatingaschildrenwhostart Meta-analyseshaveshownthatvocabularyinstruc- tionforyoungchildren,oftendesignedtobedelivered within the context of storybook reading, yields mostly aUniversityofSouthFlorida,Tampa moderateeffects(Marulis&Neuman,2010;Mol,Bus,& bUniversityofMissouri,Columbia deJong,2009;NationalEarlyLiteracyPanel,2008).Various CorrespondencetoHowardGoldstein:[email protected] approachesareusedtopromotevocabularyskillswithin Editor-in-Chief:SeanM.Redmond Editor:StephenM.Camarata Disclosure:HowardGoldsteinandElizabethKelleyareauthorsofStoryFriends ReceivedJuly5,2019 andhaveafinancialinterest,astheyreceiveroyaltiesfromsalesthroughPaul RevisionreceivedAugust13,2019 BrookesPublishing.Thisinteresthasbeenreviewedbytheiruniversitiesin AcceptedSeptember9,2019 accordancewiththeirIndividualConflictofInterestpolicies,forthepurposeof https://doi.org/10.1044/2019_JSLHR-19-00052 maintainingtheobjectivityandintegrityoftheresearch. JournalofSpeech,Language,andHearingResearch (cid:129) Vol.63 (cid:129) 173–189 (cid:129) January2020 (cid:129) Copyright©2019AmericanSpeech-Language-HearingAssociation 173 Downloaded from: https://pubs.asha.org Xigrid Soto on 03/11/2020, Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/pubs/rights_and_permissions the context of storybooks. For example, interactive afterbeingprovidedwithacurriculumandprofessional readinganddialogicreading(Lonigan&Whitehurst,1998; development,thereisevidencethattheymaycontinueto Whitehurstetal.,1988,1994)aredesignedtodeveloporal fallshortinthequalityoftheirinstruction(Justiceetal., languageinyoungchildrenthroughtheuseofpurposeful 2008).Thechallengesfacedbypreschoolteachersarefurther adult–childinteractionsandscaffoldingduringstorybook complicatedbyalackofearlychildhoodcurriculadesigned reading. Although these approaches to oral language tohelpteachersdeliverinstructionthatfostersvocabulary developmentarewidelystudiedandcommonlyused,the growthinchildren(Neuman&Dwyer,2009).Thus,there meta-analysis by Mol et al. (2009) concludes that these isaneedforwell-designed,explicitvocabularycurricula approaches are only moderately effective when imple- thatarehighlyeffectiveyeteasyforearlychildhoodprofes- mentedbyresearchersandfarlesseffectivewhenimple- sionalstoimplementwithfidelityinpreschoolsettings. mentedbytrainedteachers.MarulisandNeumanexamine Story Friends (Goldstein et al., 2016) is a supple- abroaderspectrumofapproachestovocabularyinstruction mentalcurriculumforpreschoolchildrenthatdeliversex- intheirmeta-analysis of studies with preschoolers and plicitvocabularyinstructionthroughengaging,prerecorded kindergarteners.IncontrasttoMoletal.,theseresearchers storybooks,therebymakingthecurriculuminherentlyeasy specifically examined the effects of explicit instruction to implement with fidelity. Each audio book provides onvocabulary knowledge. Their findings suggest that embeddedinstructionoffoursophisticatedwordsthatis approachesthatexplicitlyteachwordsandmeaningsand basedonseveralkeyprinciplesforrobustvocabularyin- providemultipleexamplesbefore,during,orafterstory- structiondescribedbyBeck,McKeown,andKucan(2002, bookreadingaremosteffectiveatimprovingwordlearning. 2008,2013).InStoryFriends,thetargetwordsarefirst Multiplestudiesexaminingtheuseofexplicitvocabulary presentedwithinthenarrationofthestory,andprimary instructionwithinthecontextofstorybooksfurtherdem- instructionofthetargetwordsimmediatelyfollows.The onstratethepowerofthisinstructionwhenprovidedto lessons provide a child-friendly definition and multiple young children across ability levels (Beck & McKeown, examplesusingeachwordinavarietyofcontexts.Chil- 2007; Coyne,McCoach,&Kapp,2007;Goldsteinetal., dren are given multiple opportunities to practice and to 2016;Justice, Meier, & Walpole, 2005; Loftus-Rattan, activelyrespondduringthisinstruction.Inaddition,chil- Mitchell,&Coyne,2016; Pollard-Durodolaetal.,2011). dren listen to each book three times, thereby receiving Evensmalldosesofcarefullyplanned,explicitinstruction repeatedexposuretoeachlesson. havesignificantlyimprovedorallanguageandvocabulary PriorstudieshaveshownStoryFriendstobeeffec- outcomesforat-riskchildreninpreschoolandkindergarten tiveatincreasingvocabularyknowledgeinpreschoolersat (Marulis&Neuman,2010). riskforlanguageandliteracydifficulties.Onaverage,chil- Despitetheevidencesupportingexplicitvocabulary dreninthesestudiesdemonstratelearningof30%–50%of instruction,thereisgrowingconcernthatpreschoolchildren theinstructedwords(Goldsteinetal.,2016;Greenwood receive little or no oral language instruction during the etal.,2016;Kelley&Goldstein,2014;Kelley,Goldstein, schoolday(Greenwoodetal.,2013;NationalEarlyLiteracy Spencer, & Sherman, 2015).Thisproportionofwords Panel,2008).Observationalstudiesofinstructioninearly learned compares favorably to other studies of explicit childhoodsettingsrevealthatmanypreschoolteachersfail vocabularyinterventioninwhichchildrenlearnedasmaller toprovidehigh-qualityorallanguageinstructiontochil- percentageofwordstaught(e.g.,Justiceetal.,2005;Loftus, dreninneed(Dickinson,2011;Justice,Mashburn,Hamre, Coyne,McCoach,&Zipoli,2010).Consideringtherela- &Pianta,2008;Wright,2012).Oneapproachtoincreasing tivelylowdoseofinstruction(approximately45minper thedeliveryofhigh-qualityorallanguageinstructionin week) and the limited language abilities of participants, earlychildhoodclassroomsinvolveschangingteachers’ StoryFriendsoffersanexampleofaneffectivevocabulary practices(Cabell,Justice,McGinty,DeCoster,&Forston, interventionthatcanbeimplementedinreal-worldclass- 2015;Cabelletal.,2011;Wasik,Bond,&Hindman,2006; roomssettings. Wasik&Hindman,2014).Forexample,Wasikandher AlthoughStoryFriendshasconsistentlyresultedin colleaguestrainedpreschoolteacherstoimprovetheir robusteffectsonwordlearninginyoungchildren,itispos- vocabularyinstructionandlaterobservedteachersduring siblethattheseeffectshavenotbeenmaximized.Agrow- read-alouds to code the frequency of their discussions ingbodyofresearchhasexaminedtheeffectsofextending aboutvocabulary.Theseresearchersfoundthatstudents vocabularyinstructionbeyondstorybookreading.These ofteacherswhodiscussedwordsmorefrequentlydemon- extensions,whichprovideincreasedopportunitiesforprac- stratedbettervocabularyoutcomes.Unfortunately,there tice throughout the day, have resulted in significantly stillaremanypreschoolteacherswhofinditchallenging bettervocabularylearningoutcomes(Beck&McKeown, to providehigh-quality oral languageinstruction despite 2007;Coyneetal.,2007;Coyne,McCoach,Loftus,Zipoli, access to training and instructional tools, as has been &Kapp,2009;Loftus-Rattanetal.,2016;McKeown& observedacrossearlychildhoodsettings(Dickinson,2011; Beck,2014;Wasik&Bond,2001).Inthesestudies,young Justiceetal.,2008;Pence,Justice,&Wiggins,2008).Fidelity childrenwhoreceivedextendedinstructionandpracticeof maybelowforteachersimplementinglanguagecurricula vocabularybeyondtheinstructionembeddedduringstory- inpreschools(e.g.,Dickinson,2011;Penceetal.,2008). bookreadinglearnedmorewordsthanchildrenwhodid Inaddition,evenwhenteachersareabletoachievefidelity not.Theseresultsarenotsurprisingasextensionactivities 174 JournalofSpeech,Language,andHearingResearch (cid:129) Vol.63 (cid:129) 173–189 (cid:129) January2020 Downloaded from: https://pubs.asha.org Xigrid Soto on 03/11/2020, Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/pubs/rights_and_permissions tendtoprovidemoreexposuretowordsandmeanings, teachersand49parentsofchildrenparticipatingprovided moreopportunitiestopracticethroughouttheschoolday, informedconsent.Oneconsentedchildleftthepreschool andmorecontextswithwhichchildrenareabletoconnect beforepretesting. tonewwords. Parentswhoagreedtoallowtheirchildrentopartici- Acrossstudies,extensionactivitieshavebeendesigned patewereaskedtocompleteademographicsurveyabout toincreaseexposuretowordsandmeaningsbyproviding theirfamily’ssocioeconomicstatus,thehomelanguage (a)repetitionofthesameinstructionalactivitiesacross environment,andthedevelopmentalhistoryoftheirchild. multipledays(Beck&McKeown,2007;McKeown&Beck, AllchildrenspokeEnglishastheirprimarylanguage,and 2014);(b)follow-upandreviewactivitiesthatoccurafter fourofthestudentsspokeanadditionallanguage(Spanish, bookreading(e.g.,Coyneetal.,2007);or(c)avarietyof Telugu)intheirhomes.Twenty-twoof23familiescompleted activitiesthatoccurbefore,during,andafterbookreading thedemographicsurvey.Characteristicsofthechildren (e.g.,Neuman,Newman,&Dwyer,2011;Wasik&Bond, enrolledinthestudyarepresentedinTable1.Allparents 2001).Most of these extensions have been designed to reported holding a minimumofa high school diploma provide rich opportunities for children to process the or GeneralEducationalDevelopment,12parentsheld meaningsofnewwordsmoredeeply(e.g.,Coyneetal., a bachelor’sdegree(sevenmothers,fivefathers),andeight 2007;McKeown&Beck,2014;Neuman et al., 2011). As heldagraduate/professionaldegree(fourmothers,fourfa- McKeown and Beck explain, cognitive processing that is thers).Householdsocioeconomicstatus(SES)levelswere deeper than rote memoryorwordassociationsiscritical classifiedintofivesocialstratabasedonHollingshead’s forcompleteandnuancedwordlearning.Aslearnersre- Four Factor Index of Social Status (Hollingshead,1975), peatedlyengagewithandintegratewordsinawiderange asindicatedinTable1.Thesixfamilieswithscoresbelow ofmeaningfulcontexts,richsemanticnetworksareformed 30wouldbeconsideredoflowSES.Twoofthe23children andgeneralizationoflearningisfostered. haddiagnosed disabilities (onewith cerebralpalsy,the The purpose of the current study was to evaluate otherunreported).Allparticipantswereeligibletoattend ClasswideVocabularyReviewStrategies(CVRS),anex- kindergartenthefollowingyear. tensiontotheStoryFriendscurriculum.Thisinstructional Allchildrenwhoseparentsconsentedwerescreened componentwasdesignedtoincreasevocabularylearningin todeterminetheireligibility.Atotalof48childrenfrom preschoolchildrenreceivingtheStoryFriendssmall-group fourclassroomswereinitiallyscreenedwiththreescreening explicit vocabulary instruction. Teachers were provided measures:(a)thePictureNamingSubtestoftheIndividual withpromptsandmaterialstoencouragereviewofand Growth and Development Indicators (IGDI; Wackerle- opportunitiestopracticevocabularywordsduringroutine Hollman,McConnell,&Rodriguez,2017), (b) the Peabody classroomactivitiesthroughouttheschoolday.Thefollow- PictureVocabularyTest–FourthEdition(PPVT-IV;Dunn ingresearchquestionswereaddressed: &Dunn,2007),and(c)theCoreLanguagesubtests ofthe ClinicalEvaluationofLanguageFundamentalsPreschool– 1. TowhatextentdoestheadditionofCVRStothe SecondEdition(CELFPreschool-2;Wiig,Secord,&Semel, StoryFriendsprogramimprovevocabularylearning 2004).Thesetools areindividuallyadministered,norm- comparedtotheStoryFriendsprogramalonefor referenced measures of expressivevocabulary,receptive childrenwithbelow-averageorallanguageskillsat vocabulary,andorallanguageskills.TheIGDIPicture riskforreadingdifficulties? Namingsubtestwasadministeredtoidentifychildrenwith 2. TowhatextentdotheCVRSresultinvocabulary significantlanguagedelays(lackingincorevocabulary)and learningamongabove-averagechildrenwhoarenot thosewhowereexceeding expectations.Thus,children exposedtothesmall-groupvocabularyinstruction? withRausch-basedscoresbetween46and52,indicating 3. Towhatextentdoteachersocialvalidityresultsand poortomoderateorallanguageskillsrelativetoanorma- proceduralfidelitymeasuresattesttotheusability tivesample,wereidentifiedforfurtherlanguagetesting. andfeasibilityofStoryFriendsandCVRSprogram Childrenwereselectediftheirstandardscoreswereatthe components? normtonomorethan1.5SDsbelowthemeanoneither thePPVT-IVortheCELFPreschool-2tobeincludedas primaryparticipants.Participantsdemonstratedmeanscores Method on thePPVT-IV,CELF Preschool-2,andIGDIof 93.0 (SD=7.2),90.7(SD=6.67),and48.7(SD=2.23),respec- Participants tively.Children’sperformances on these measuresare Four preschool teachers and 23 childrenwere re- provided in Table1.Threeof 26selectedchildren were cruitedfromfourstate-subsidized,VoluntaryPrekindergarten excludedfromthestudyduetolowattendance. (VPK) classrooms serving children primarily from low- Tenchildrenwithconsistentattendancerecordsfrom incomehouseholds.Thefourfemaleclassroomteachers twoofthelargerVPKclassroomswereinitiallyscreened ranged from 31 to 50 years of age; two were multiracial, foreligibilityandexcludedduetohighPPVTscores(M= onewasAfricanAmerican,andonewasHispanic.The 110,SD=4.7).Thesechildrendidnotparticipateinthe teachershad3–15yearsofteachingexperience;eachattended small-groupinterventionbutwereexposedtotheCVRS. somecollege,withonereceivingherassociatedegree.Four By testing thesechildren at posttest,wesought to gain Sevenetal.:ClasswideExtensionsofVocabularyIntervention 175 Downloaded from: https://pubs.asha.org Xigrid Soto on 03/11/2020, Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/pubs/rights_and_permissions Table1.Childandfamilycharacteristics. Class Child Gender Age SESscoresa Ethnicity PPVT CELF IGDI_PN A 01.b F 4–8 46 Asian 101 94 52. 02 F 4–11 53 AfricanAmerican 96 94 47. 03 F 3–11 43.5 AfricanAmerican 97 90 48. 04 F 4–10 42.5 Caucasian 101 102 50. 05 M 4–3 66 Caucasian 97 79 47. 06.b F 4–1 66 Asian 101 100 50. B 07 M 4–0 AfricanAmerican 101 84 47. 08 M 4–7 33 AfricanAmerican 91 90 48. 09.b F 4–1 37 Hispanic/Latino 96 100 50. 10 F 4–6 27 AfricanAmerican 97 86 53. 11 M 4–7 13 AfricanAmerican 100 90 47. C 12 F 5–5 53 Multiracial 85 79 53. 13 F 5–5 53 Multiracial 92 94 14 M 4–9 53 Multiracial 88 90 48. 15 M 4–9 53 AfricanAmerican 84 84 47. 16 M 5–5 AfricanAmerican 94 100 50. 17 M 4–9 48 AfricanAmerican 84 86 46. D 18 M 3–10 14 AfricanAmerican 95 86 48. 19 M 4–5 25 AfricanAmerican 76 88 46. 20 F 4–4 28 AfricanAmerican 82 86 48. 21.b F 5–0 34.5 Hispanic/Latino 99 100 52. 22 F 4–9 <8 AfricanAmerican 94 94 48. 23 F 4–5 32 AfricanAmerican 87 90 46. M 92.96 90.70 48.68 SD 7.12 6.67 2.23 Note. SES=socioeconomicstatus;PPVT-IV=PeabodyPictureVocabularyTest–FourthEdition;CELF=ClinicalEvaluationofLanguage FundamentalsPreschool–SecondEdition,CoreLanguageSubtest;IGDI_PN=IndividualGrowthandDevelopmentIndicatorPictureNaming Subtest;F=female;M=male. aSESscoreswerecalculatedbasedonHollingshead’sFourFactorIndexofSocialStatus(Hollingshead,1975).Scoresof55–66areinthe majorbusinessandprofessionalstrata,scoresof40–54areinthemediumbusinessandminorprofessionalstrata,scoresof30–39areinthe skilledcraftsmenandclericalstrata,scoresof20–29areinthesemiskilledworkersstrata,andscoresof8–19areintheunskilledworkers strata.bFamiliesofthesechildrenreportedthatchildrenspoketwolanguagesathome.Child1andChild6spokeTeleguandEnglish,and Child9andChild21spokeSpanishandEnglishathome. preliminaryinformationontheextentofvocabularylearning TheintroductorybookintroducesJungleFriendscharac- thatoccurredasafunctionoftheCVRSalone(i.e.,with- tersandtheprocedureofinteractivebook-readingsessions. outsmall-groupinstruction). EachJungleFriendsinstructionalbookincludesinterac- tiveembeddedlessonstoexplicitlyteachfourchallenging Setting words(e.g.,enormous,soar),whichwouldqualifyasTier2 vocabularyaccordingtoBecketal.(2013).Thesevocabu- Assessments,screening,andinterventiontookplace larywordswereselectedbasedontheirfrequencyinadult atfourVPKchildcarecenters.Thesecentersweresituated language,theirutilityforacademiclanguageinstruction, inhigh-povertyneighborhoods.Thesmall-groupStory their relevance to the storyline, their applicability to a Friendslisteningcentersessionstookplaceinsmallquiet varietyofcontexts,andchildren’slackoffamiliaritywith roomsasdesignatedbyschooladministrators. Intervention Components and Materials Table2.ListofJungleFriendsbooksusedinthisstudy. AutomatedListeningCenters TheStoryFriendsprogramincludesautomatedlis- NJF NewJungleFriend teningcentersdeliveredinsmallgroupsandfacilitatedby EFD Ellie’sFirstDay anadult.Childrenlistentoprerecordedstorybookswith MMA MarquezMonkeyAround MBD Marquez’sBackwardsDay embedded,explicitlessonstoteachchallengingvocabulary EGS EllieGetsStuck words.ListeningcentermaterialsincludeStoryFriends ECF IfElephantsCouldFly storybooks,mp3playerswithaccompanyingaudiofiles,an LBF Leo’sBraveFace audiosplitter,andheadphones.Thestorybooksetforthis LLR LeoLoseshisRoar studyincludedoneintroductorybook(MeettheJungle Note. Theorderofthebookswascounterbalancedbetween Friends)and eight instructional books from the Jungle classrooms. Friendsbookseries.ThebooktitlesarepresentedinTable2. 176 JournalofSpeech,Language,andHearingResearch (cid:129) Vol.63 (cid:129) 173–189 (cid:129) January2020 Downloaded from: https://pubs.asha.org Xigrid Soto on 03/11/2020, Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/pubs/rights_and_permissions thetargetwords(basedonpriortestingwithpreschoolers). researchstaffandteachersduringthestudy.Attheendof Eachtargetvocabularywordistaughtintwoembedded training,researchstaffsharedtheclassroomextension lessons per book that include systematic instructional materialsandhelpedteacherssetthemupintheclassroom. language.Duringeachlesson,childrenareprovidedwith threeopportunitiestosaytheword,twoopportunitiesto StoryFriendsListeningCenters saythedefinition,anexampleofthewordinthecontext TrainedresearchstafffacilitatedtheStoryFriends ofthestory,anexampleofthewordinacommonlife experience,andanactivityrelatedtotheword’smeaning. listeningcentersthreetimesaweek.Theroleof thefacili- tatorwastoensurethateachchildhadaheadphoneand Thechildrenrespondtothebooknarratorandspeakout a book and to encourage children to participate (e.g., loud while wearing headphones; the adult monitoring respondingtothenarrator’sinstructions)andtostayon looksonexpectantlyandpromptschildrenonoccasion. task(e.g.,turningthepages).Thefacilitatorprovidedpraise Childrenlisten to each book three times per week.Each foron-taskbehaviorandredirectedchildrenwhenneces- Story Friends prerecorded audio includes four target vocabularywordsthatare10–12mininlength.Intotal, sary but did not provide any additional instruction or explicitapprovalofthechildren’sresponses.Implementa- childrenspentlessthan40mininsmallgroupseachweek. tionoflisteningcentersbyresearchstaffensuredthatthe teacherswereblindtowordsintroducedinthebooksin CVRS theStoryFriends–onlycontrolcondition. ThepurposeofCVRSistopromptteacherstopro- Children participated in the listening centers in videopportunitiestoreviewandpracticetheuseoftarget groupsofthreeorfourwiththefacilitator.Priortolisten- vocabularywordsincontextsbeyondtheiruseinStory ingtoanyoftheinstructionalbooks,participantslistened Friendsbooks.Thematerialsaredesignedtobeeasyto totheStoryFriendsintroductorybook(MeettheJungle use,flexible,andreadilyincorporatedintoawiderangeof Friends)onetime.Theintroductorysessionenabledthem typicalclassroomroutines. topracticelisteningcenterexpectationsandprocedures, AspartoftheCVRScondition,teacherswereprompted suchasrespondingtotheinteractivelessonsandturning toreviewtarget wordswithtext messageremindersand pageswhendirected. in-classvisualremindersandmaterials.Dailytextmessage Inbothconditions,childrenparticipatedinsmall- remindersmodeledfivesentencesperweekthatcouldbe groupcentersthreetimesperweek.Promptsandmaterials usedinvariousclassroomconversations.Thesesentences usedintheCVRSconditionwereintroducedeveryother includedphrasesthatrelatedtochildren’sexperiences,or week.DuringtheCVRScondition,teacherspracticedtarget thatofferedfurtherexplanationsofwords,orpromptsfor vocabularywordsfromagivenbookduringtheweekthe childrentocompletesentencesorrespondtorequestsor bookwasintroduced,andtheycontinuedtopracticethe questions(e.g.,“Youarebirds!LiftupyourwingsandSoar samevocabularywordsforasecondweekaschildrenlis- tothedoor!”).Thesetextmessagesweresenttotheteachers tenedtoanewbookinthelisteningcenter–onlycondition. attheirpreferredtimeoftheday.Visualremindersand materialsincludedwordcards,aweeklywordchart,anda reviewboard.Thewordcardsweredesignedtoremind Dependent Variables teacherstousephraseswithtargetvocabularywordsduring schooltime.TheweeklywordchartincludedVelcro-backed Children’sknowledgeoftargetedvocabularywords cards with the Tier 2 words and meanings to remind wasassessedusingamasterymonitoringprobe.Mastery teacherstousethetargetwordsandhelpthemkeeptrack monitoringvocabularyprobesintermixedtestingofwords oftheirfrequencyofworduse.Thereviewboardfunctioned tobetaughtwithwordsthatwerepreviouslytaught.Words asawordwalltopromptreviewofpreviouslytaughttarget were each assessed at four time points: pretest (before vocabularywordsandincludedsmallpicturesfromthe listening to storybooks), posttest (after completing three storyaswellasreal-lifepictures(seeFigure1). listeningcenters),Maintenance1(1weekafterposttest), andMaintenance2(2weeksafterposttest).Thus,testing occurredonfoursuccessiveweeksforeachbook.During Procedure themasterymonitorprobes,childrenwereaskedtopro- The duration of the study from teacher training vide definitions for targeted vocabulary words (e.g., through the administration of maintenance probes was Whatdoessoarmean?).Thechildresponseswerescored 11weeks.Priortoimplementation,classroomteachers asreflectingcompleteknowledge(2points),partialknowl- received a brief training (~60 min) in a quiet room to edge (1 point), and no knowledge (0 points), with a discusstheprogramprocedureandexpectations.Thefollow- maximumscoreof8forfourtargetvocabularywordsper ingtopicswerepresentedatthemeeting:(a)thebenefits book. ofwordlearningonchildren’sacademicsuccess,(b)sched- Socialvalidityassessmentswereconductedtoevaluate uling and situating Story Friends listening centers for self-reportedteacherperceptionsregardingtheirimpressions threesessionseachweek,(c)therationaleofStoryFriends andsatisfactionwiththeprogramanditsfeasibility.The CVRS and howto makeuseof thematerials provided, twoassessmentsincludedastructuredinterviewwithseven and (d)thedailyexpectations,requirements,androlesfor open-endedquestionsandasurveywithnineLikert-scale Sevenetal.:ClasswideExtensionsofVocabularyIntervention 177 Downloaded from: https://pubs.asha.org Xigrid Soto on 03/11/2020, Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/pubs/rights_and_permissions Figure1.ComponentsofClasswideVocabularyReviewStrategies. questionsthatevaluatedteachers’attitudesandideasre- pretest,100%atposttest,93.75%atMaintenance1,and gardingtheStoryFriendscurriculum. 96.87%atMaintenance2probes. Researchstaffwhofacilitatedthelisteningcenters collectedfieldnotesduringschoolvisitsthatoverlappedwith Experimental Design and Analysis classrooms’circleandcentertimes.Thesefieldnoteswere notcollectedsystematically,butwetookadvantageofeight AsinpriorevaluationsofStoryFriendsintervention, 20-minopportunitiestogatherexamplesofhowteachers arepeatedacquisitiondesignwasusedtodeterminethe wereaddressingtargetwordsintheclassroom.Theobservers extenttowhichvocabularylearningeffectswerereplicated transcribedtheseteacher–childconversationsverbatim. within and across participants for each book. The crite- rionfordemonstratingexperimentaleffectsrequiredagain from before intervention to after intervention of at least Fidelity of Assessment Administration 2wordpoints(e.g.,theequivalentoflearningonewordor and Scoring Reliability demonstratingpartialknowledgeoftwowords)foreach The research staff received training on all mea- book.Thus,wecalculatedthepercentageofbooksforwhich sures.This training covered administration of the CELF learningeffectswerereplicated. Preschool-2, PPVT-IV, and IGDI Picture Naming stan- Toanswerourprimaryresearchquestionforthis dardizedassessmenttools,aswellastheresearcher-developed study,anadaptedalternatingtreatmentsdesign(AATD) masterymonitorvocabularyprobes.Masterymonitorvo- wasusedtocomparetheeffectsofStoryFriendslistening cabulary assessmentsessions wererecorded to examine centerinstructionalone(SF)versusStoryFriendslistening thefidelityoftest administration.A second research staff centerinstructionwithCVRS.TheAATDwasselected member listenedto20%ofalltestingsessionsandjudged because of its applicability to comparing the effects of whetheritemswerereadcorrectly,responseswererecorded instructionalpracticeswithnonreversiblebehaviors(Gast& accurately,and thestandardprompting protocolwas Ledford,2014).Theorderofthetwoconditionswascoun- followed.Fidelityofassessmentadministrationaveraged terbalancedacrossclassroomstocontrolforpossiblediffer- 89%(range:71%–98%). encesinworddifficultyamongbooks.TheSFcondition Afteraprimarytrainedresearcherscoredallmea- includedthreelisteningcentersessionsandlasted1week sures,asecondtrainedstaffmemberrescored20%ofthe foreachbook.TheCVRSconditionalsoincludedlisten- masterymonitorvocabularyassessmentstoassessreliabil- ingcenterslasting1weekandaddedteacher-implemented ity.Item-by-itemresponseagreementwascalculatedby classroomvocabularyreviewstrategiesthatoccurredfor dividingtheagreementsbythetotalnumberofagreement 2weeks.Thesetwoconditionswereinitiatedinanalternat- anddisagreements.Interrateragreementpercentageswere ingfashionwitheachnewbook,andbookassignmentwas high, averaging 96.9% (range: 93.75%–100): 100% at counterbalancedamongclassrooms.Thus,fourbookswere 178 JournalofSpeech,Language,andHearingResearch (cid:129) Vol.63 (cid:129) 173–189 (cid:129) January2020 Downloaded from: https://pubs.asha.org Xigrid Soto on 03/11/2020, Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/pubs/rights_and_permissions assignedtotheSFcondition,andfourbookswereassigned for fourtarget vocabulary wordsper bookbeforeinter- totheCVRScondition,exceptforClassB,whichwith- vention(pretest),afterintervention(posttest),andafterthe drewfromthestudyaftersixbooksbecausetheclassroom secondandthirdweeks(Maintenance1and2).Thus,vo- teachertransferredtoanotherpreschool. cabularylearningscoreswereplottedatfourtimepoints Visualanalyses,descriptivestatistics,multilevelmodel- foreachbook,withclosedtrianglesdenotingtheStory ingprocedures,andsummariesofsocialvalidityandpro- Friends–only (SF) condition and closed circles denoting ceduralfidelitymeasureswereusedtoevaluatetreatment theStoryFriends+CVRScondition.Theexpectedlearning effects.First,visualanalysisallowedresearcherstodeter- trendwasanabruptchangeinlevelafterintroducingthe minewhetherfunctionalrelationsbetweentreatmentsand SFintervention.However,higherlevelsandgreaterstabil- vocabularylearningweredemonstrated,whetherlearning itywereexpectedfortheSF+CVRScondition. wasenhancedbyCVRS,andtheextenttowhichresults First,usingtherepeatedacquisitiondesign,weevalu- were replicated within and across participants (Gast & atedwhethertherewererepeatedimprovementsfrom Ledford,2014). pretesttoposttests/maintenancetestsasbookswereintro- Second,vocabularygainswerecomputedforeach ducedeachweek.The23childrenwereexposedtoasmany studentforeachoftheeightbooksbysubtractingpretest as eight books each, resulting in a total of 174 possible scoresfromtheaverageofvocabularyscoresforthepost- replications.Learningeffectsofanincreaseofatleast2 test,Maintenance1,andMaintenance2.Thesegainscores wordpointswasdemonstratedfor89%ofthe174possible wereinitiallyanalyzeddescriptivelybycomputingleast replications.Becausetherewere3weeksofposttestingand squareestimates,thatis,meangainacrossbooksforeach maintenancetestingforeachbook,variabilityinperfor- experimentalcondition(SFandCVRS)foreachparticipant. mancecanbeseen(seeFigures2–4).Mostoften,theper- Third,atwo-level,linear,cross-classified,random formancestayed relatively consistent with theposttest. effectsmodelingprocedurewasusedtofurthercharacter- Youwillseeexamplesofdrop-offs,stability,andincreases izevariationsinchildren’svocabularygains.Althoughthe inlearninggainsinboththeSFandCVRSconditions, automated nature of SF listening centers results in high withmoreexamplesofcontinuedgrowthintheCVRS treatmentfidelity,teachersaremorelikelytovaryinthe condition.Interestingly,alackoflearningeffectswasdem- extenttowhichtheyimplementCVRS.Becausechildren onstrated17timesforbooksduringtheSFconditionand arenestedinclassrooms,multilevelmodelingwasapplied. onlythreetimesduringtheCVRScondition. Morespecifically,fixedeffectsinthemodelallowedusto Second, the first and last authors independently estimateandtesttheaveragedifferenceingainsbetween judgedwhethertheAATDrevealeddifferentialexperimen- thetwotreatmentconditions(CVRSandSF),aswellasto taleffectsforeachparticipantwhencomparingtheSFand estimatedifferencesingainsbetweentheclassrooms,which CVRSconditions.Theywereinagreementfor91%ofthe wetreatedasfixedeffectsbecausetherewerelessthanfive participants.Figures2–4areorganizedaccordingtothe classrooms,andtotesttheclassroombytreatmenteffects. agreed-uponresultsof thisvisualanalysis.Figure2pre- Therandomeffectsinthemodelallowedustoexamine sents12childrenwhodemonstratedoverallhigherperfor- thevariationinthegainsacrossbooksandchildren.The manceintheCVRSconditionthanintheSFcondition; cross-classifiedrandomeffectsmodelinganalyseswerecon- Figure3presentsthreechildrenwhodidnotshowdifferen- ductedusingSPSSmixedmodelswithrestrictedmaximum tialeffectsbecauseofceilingeffects(i.e.,veryhighvocabu- likelihoodestimation.Satterthwaiteestimateddegreesof larylearninginbothconditions);andFigure4presents freedomwereappliedtoobtainfixed-effectinferencesthat eightchildren,sixofwhomshowedinconsistentdifferential have been shown to be accurate with single-case data effectsbetweenconditionsandtwochildren(P18andP19) (Ferron,Bell,Hess,Rendina-Gobioff,&Hibbard,2009; whoshowedweaklearningeffectsingeneral. Ferron,Farmer,&Owens,2010). Weaveragedthegainsforeachofthebooksassigned Finally, social validity, procedural fidelity, field toeachconditiontoobtainthemeangainforeachchild notes,andteacher-reportedimplementationdataprovided (seeTable3).AveragechildvocabularygainsintheCVRS richdatasourcestoevaluatetheteacherperceptionsandto conditionwere4.7wordpoints(SD=1.37)versusamean gather examples of teacher-improvised implementation. of2.8wordpoints(SD=1.16)intheSFcondition,adif- Aftercompilingandorganizingthequalitativedatafrom ferenceof1.9wordpoints.Averagechildvocabularygains various resources, the common themes were identified alsovariedamongclassrooms.AscanbeseeninTable3, throughopencodingandcodecondensation(Creswell& ClassroomsA,B,C,andDshoweddifferencesinmean Poth,2017). gainscoresbetweentheCVRSandSFconditionsof0.6, 2.5,1.7,and3.0wordpoints,respectively.Basedonvisual inspectionoftheAATD,weidentified12childrenwho Results demonstratedhigherperformanceintheCVRScondition; theiraveragegainovertheSFconditionwas2.86word Comparing Vocabulary Gains Across Conditions points.Interclasscorrelationcoefficientvarianceproportions Visualanalysisoftheimmediacy,level,variability, werecalculatedfortheunconditionalmodel.Theinterclass andmaintenanceofeffectswasconductedfor23graphs correlationcoefficientsinthisanalysiswere.31forchildren presentedinFigures2–4.Datapointsreflectcorrectresponses and.049forbooks.Hence,atwo-level,linear,cross-classified, Sevenetal.:ClasswideExtensionsofVocabularyIntervention 179 Downloaded from: https://pubs.asha.org Xigrid Soto on 03/11/2020, Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/pubs/rights_and_permissions Figure2.ResultsofrepeatedacquisitionandadaptedalternatingtreatmentsdesignsshowingsuperiorwordlearningfortheClasswide VocabularyReviewStrategiescondition(solidcircles)versustheStoryFriends–onlycondition(solidtriangles).Thex-axisrepresentsthe orderofabbreviatedbooknamesinTable2.Foreachbook,fourdatapointsareshownforpretest,postintervention,MaintenanceWeek1, andMaintenanceWeek2.NJF=NewJungleFriend;EFD=Ellie’sFirstDay;MBD=Marquez’sBackwardsDay;MMA=MarquezMonkeyAround; EGS=EllieGetsStuck;ECF=IfElephantsCouldFly;LBF=Leo’sBraveFace;LLR=LeoLoseshisRoar. randomeffectsmodelwasusedtodisentanglehowthevocab- ularygains,whichwerecross-classifiedby23childrenand β ¼γ þγ (cid:2)ðClassAÞþγ (cid:2)ðClassBÞþγ 1jk 10 11 12 13 eightbooks,variedasafunctionofexperimentalcondition (cid:2)ðClassCÞþν þu 1k 1j (Level2),(3) (CVRSorSF)andclassroom.Morespecifically, WORDKNOWijk ¼β0jkþβ1jkTreatmentþrijk whereWORDKNOWijkrepresentsithvocabularygainfor (Level1),(1) thejthchildandthekthbook,treatmentiscoded1 for CVRS and 0 for SF, and the Class variables are coded1 β0jk ¼γ00þγ01(cid:2)ðClassAÞþγ02(cid:2)ðClassBÞþγ03 fortheclassindicatedand0forallotherclasses.Thus,γ00 (cid:2)ðClassCÞþν þu (Level2),(2) istheaveragegaininClassDfortheSFcondition,and 0k 0j 180 JournalofSpeech,Language,andHearingResearch (cid:129) Vol.63 (cid:129) 173–189 (cid:129) January2020 Downloaded from: https://pubs.asha.org Xigrid Soto on 03/11/2020, Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/pubs/rights_and_permissions Figure3.ResultsofrepeatedacquisitionandadaptedalternatingtreatmentsdesignsshowingceilingeffectsfortheClasswideVocabulary ReviewStrategiescondition(solidcircles)andtheStoryFriends–onlycondition(solidtriangles).Thex-axisrepresentstheorderof abbreviatedbooknames.Foreachbook,fourdatapointsareshownforpretest,postintervention,MaintenanceWeek1,andMaintenance Week2.NJF=NewJungleFriend;EFD=Ellie’sFirstDay;MBD=Marquez’sBackwardsDay;MMA=MarquezMonkeyAround;EGS=Ellie GetsStuck;ECF=IfElephantsCouldFly;LBF=Leo’sBraveFace;LLR=LeoLoseshisRoar. Figure4.Resultsofrepeatedacquisitionandadaptedalternatingtreatmentsdesignsshowingnodifferentialorminimalwordlearningforthe ClasswideVocabularyReviewStrategiescondition(solidcircles)andtheStoryFriends–onlycondition(solidtriangles).Thex-axisrepresents theorderofabbreviatedbooknames.Foreachbook,fourdatapointsareshownforpretest,postintervention,MaintenanceWeek1,and MaintenanceWeek2.NJF=NewJungleFriend;EFD=Ellie’sFirstDay;MBD=Marquez’sBackwardsDay;MMA=MarquezMonkey Around;EGS=EllieGetsStuck;ECF=IfElephantsCouldFly;LBF=Leo’sBraveFace;LLR=LeoLoseshisRoar. Sevenetal.:ClasswideExtensionsofVocabularyIntervention 181 Downloaded from: https://pubs.asha.org Xigrid Soto on 03/11/2020, Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/pubs/rights_and_permissions Table3.Meansandstandarddeviationsofchildvocabularygainsbyexperimentalcondition. StoryFriendsplusClasswide VocabularyReviewStrategies StoryFriendsonly Meandifference ChildID M SD M SD betweenconditions 01a 5.50 1.66 6.25 0.43 −0.75 02b 5.67 1.51 2.25 0.72 3.42 03c 3.33 1.20 2.92 1.72 0.41 04a 4.33 1.39 5.50 1.50 −1.17 05c 4.75 1.11 4.58 2.19 0.17 06b 5.58 1.42 4.25 1.82 1.33 ClassroomA 4.86 1.38 4.29 1.40 0.57 07b 4.89 0.31 2.78 1.34 2.11 08b 2.67 1.41 1.00 0.82 1.67 09b 6.33 2.81 2.44 2.20 3.89 10b 5.56 2.64 2.89 0.83 2.67 11b 4.11 1.99 2.00 1.63 2.11 ClassroomB 4.71 1.83 2.22 1.36 2.49 12c 7.00 1.73 4.42 1.09 2.58 13a 6.17 1.28 5.75 1.38 0.42 14c 3.42 1.21 2.25 0.64 1.17 15b 3.67 0.33 1.58 0.98 2.09 16c 5.50 1.66 4.08 1.71 1.42 17b 3.50 1.09 0.83 0.87 2.67 ClassroomC 4.88 1.22 3.15 1.11 1.73 18c 1.71 1.14 0.83 1.26 0.88 19c 1.92 1.92 0.42 0.28 1.50 20b 3.75 1.75 0.75 0.36 3.00 21b 6.75 0.60 3.17 1.21 3.58 22c 6.67 0.91 3.58 1.14 3.09 23b 6.33 0.53 0.50 0.55 5.83 ClassroomD 4.52 1.14 1.54 0.80 2.98 Grandmean 4.74 1.37 2.83 1.16 1.92 Note. Italicizedvaluesindicatesummaryparametersforeachclassroom. aChildrenwhodemonstratedhighvocabularyknowledgeinbothconditions.Therangeofmeandifferencescoresofconditionsare−1.17to 0.42,averaging−0.50.bChildrenwhodemonstratedoverallhighervocabularyknowledgeintheClasswideVocabularyReviewStrategies conditionthantheStoryFriends–onlycondition.Therangeofmeandifferencescoresofconditionsare1.33–5.83,averaging2.86. cChildrenwhodemonstratedinconsistentperformancebetweenconditions.Therangeofmeandifferencescoresofconditionsare0.17–3.08, averaging1.40. thedifferences in this gain forClassesA, B, and Care AsshowninTable4,theestimatedparameterrepre- γ ,γ , and γ ,respectively. Thedifferencein average sentingchildvocabularygainsfortheSFconditionaver- 01 02 03 gainbetweentheCVRSandSFconditionsforClassD aged 1.50 word points for the reference classroom (i.e., is γ , whereas Classes A, B, and C differ from ClassD ClassroomD).ForClassroomsA,B,andC,theaverage 10 intheeffectofCVRSrelativetoSFbyγ γ ,andγ , gainswereestimatedtobe2.60,0.81,and1.49wordpoints 11, 12 13 respectively.Theerrorterms(i.e.,r ,ν ,u ,ν ,andu ) higher,respectively.Thus,theaveragegainsforthefour ijk 0k 0j 1k 1j areassumedtobenormallydistributed. classroomsfortheSFconditionwere4.10,2.31,2.99,and Theproposedmodelstatisticallyfitsbetterthanan 1.50 word points. The classroom extension condition unconditionalmodel,χ2=(11,N=174)=161.8,p<.01. expandedchildvocabularygainsbyanestimated2.80word In addition, the model assumptions were evaluated by pointsforClassroomD,whichwasastatisticallysignifi- examiningeachoftheresidualsinthemodelusingabox cantdifference,t(24)=4.75,p<.001.Whenfactoringin plotforoutliers,ascatterplotforhomoscedasticity,anda theClass×Treatmentinteractions,theaveragegainsfor QQ-plotfornormality.Resultsindicatedahomoscedastic theCVRSconditionwere4.80,4.80,4.96,and4.30for residualdistributionwithmultivariatenormalityandinde- ClassroomsA,B,C,andD,respectively. pendenceacrosslevelsandthatobservationswereindepen- AsseeninTable4,themodelalsorevealedseveral dentconditionalonthevariancecomponents. statisticallysignificantvariancecomponents.Namely,the 182 JournalofSpeech,Language,andHearingResearch (cid:129) Vol.63 (cid:129) 173–189 (cid:129) January2020 Downloaded from: https://pubs.asha.org Xigrid Soto on 03/11/2020, Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/pubs/rights_and_permissions

See more

The list of books you might like

Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.