ARTICLE IN PRESS Appetite50(2008)1–11 www.elsevier.com/locate/appet Research Review How emotions affect eating: A five-way model Michael Macht DepartmentofPsychology,UniversityofWu¨rzburg,Marcusstr.9-11,97070Wu¨rzburg,Germany Received18April2006;receivedinrevisedform13July2007;accepted16July2007 Abstract Despite the importance of affective processes in eating behaviour, it remains difficult to predict how emotions affect eating. Emphasizingindividualdifferences,previousresearchdidnotpayfullattentiontothetwofoldvariabilityofemotion-inducedchangesof eating (variability across both individuals and emotions). By contrast, the present paper takes into account both individual characteristicsandemotionfeatures,andspecifiesfiveclassesofemotion-inducedchangesofeating:(1)emotionalcontroloffoodchoice, (2)emotionalsuppressionoffoodintake,(3)impairmentofcognitiveeatingcontrols,(4)eatingtoregulateemotions,and(5)emotion- congruentmodulationofeating.Theseclassesaredistinguishedbyantecedentconditions,eatingresponsesandmediatingmechanisms. They point to basic functional principles underlying the relations between emotions and biologically based motives: interference, concomitance and regulation. Thus, emotion-induced changes of eating can be a result of interference of eating by emotions, a by- productofemotions,andaconsequenceofregulatoryprocesses(i.e.,emotionsmayregulateeating,andeatingmayregulateemotions). r2007Elsevier Ltd. Allrights reserved. Keywords:Emotion;Eating;Mood;Emotionaleating;Restrainedeating;Motivation;Foodchoice Contents Introduction . . .. .. .. .. .. .. . .. .. .. .. .. .. .. . .. .. .. .. .. .. .. . .. .. .. .. .. .. . .. .. .. .. .. .. .. . .. .. .. .. 1 Variability of emotion-inducedchanges of eating. .. . .. .. .. .. .. .. .. . .. .. .. .. .. .. . .. .. .. .. .. .. .. . .. .. .. .. 2 Variability acrossindividuals.. .. . .. .. .. .. .. .. .. . .. .. .. .. .. .. . .. .. .. .. .. .. .. . .. .. .. .. .. .. .. . ..2 Variability acrossemotions . .. .. . .. .. .. .. .. .. .. . .. .. .. .. .. .. . .. .. .. .. .. .. .. . .. .. .. .. .. .. .. . ..3 Previous theories. .. .. .. .. .. . .. .. .. .. .. .. .. . .. .. .. .. .. .. .. . .. .. .. .. .. .. . .. .. .. .. .. .. .. . .. .. .. .. 4 A five-waymodel .. .. .. .. .. . .. .. .. .. .. .. .. . .. .. .. .. .. .. .. . .. .. .. .. .. .. . .. .. .. .. .. .. .. . .. .. .. .. 4 Food-induced emotions controlfoodchoice .. .. .. .. . .. .. .. .. .. .. . .. .. .. .. .. .. .. . .. .. .. .. .. .. .. . ..5 Intense emotionssuppress foodintake .. .. .. .. .. .. . .. .. .. .. .. .. . .. .. .. .. .. .. .. . .. .. .. .. .. .. .. . ..5 Negative and positive emotionsimpair cognitive eating controls. .. .. .. . .. .. .. .. .. .. .. . .. .. .. .. .. .. .. . ..6 Negative emotions elicit eating to regulate emotions. .. . .. .. .. .. .. .. . .. .. .. .. .. .. .. . .. .. .. .. .. .. .. . ..6 Emotions modulate eatingin congruence withemotionfeatures . .. .. .. . .. .. .. .. .. .. .. . .. .. .. .. .. .. .. . ..7 Discussion .. . .. .. .. .. .. .. . .. .. .. .. .. .. .. . .. .. .. .. .. .. .. . .. .. .. .. .. .. . .. .. .. .. .. .. .. . .. .. .. .. 7 Emotion-induced eating in restrained andemotional eaters. .. .. .. .. .. . .. .. .. .. .. .. .. . .. .. .. .. .. .. .. . ..7 Emotion-induced eating in normaleaters .. .. .. .. .. . .. .. .. .. .. .. . .. .. .. .. .. .. .. . .. .. .. .. .. .. .. . ..8 References .. . .. .. .. .. .. .. . .. .. .. .. .. .. .. . .. .. .. .. .. .. .. . .. .. .. .. .. .. . .. .. .. .. .. .. .. . .. .. .. .. 8 Introduction Emotions serve distinct motivational functions and E-mailaddress:[email protected] contribute essentially to control of basic behavioural 0195-6663/$-seefrontmatterr2007ElsevierLtd.Allrightsreserved. doi:10.1016/j.appet.2007.07.002 ARTICLE IN PRESS 2 M.Macht/Appetite50(2008)1–11 systems in animals and men (e.g. Frederickson, 1998; bility of emotion-induced changes of eating is summarized Frijda, 1986; Izard & Ackerman, 2000; Plutchik, 1984; briefly. Tomkins, 1963). Specific emotions such as anger, fear, sadness, and joy, as well as moods that are thought to last Variability of emotion-induced changes of eating longerandbemorediffuse(e.g.Ekman,1992;Frijda,1993) havebeenfoundtoaffecteatingresponsesalongtheentire Variability across individuals process of ingestion: motivation to eat (Macht & Simons, 2000), affective responses to foods (Ferber & Cabanac, Aseriesofexperimentsshowedthatrestrainedeaters(as 1987; Willner & Healy, 1994), food choice (e.g. Gibson, identified by questionnaire; e.g. Herman & Mack, 1975; 2006; Oliver & Wardle, 1999), chewing (Macht, 1998), Stunkard & Messick, 1985) consume more food than non- eating speed (Krebs, Macht, Weyers, Weijers, & Janke, restrained eaters in response to fear and negative mood 1996), amount ingested (Greeno & Wing, 1994 for a states (for a review, see Greeno & Wing, 1994, also review), as well as metabolism and digestion (Blair, Wing, Heatherton, Striepe, & Wittenberg, 1998; Rotenberg & &Wald,1991;Wing,Blair,Epstein,&McDermott,1990). Flood,1999;Rutledge&Linden,1998;Tuschen,Florin,& Surveys demonstrate that most people experience changes Baucke, 1993). Restrained eating refers to a persistent of eating in response to emotional stress (on average 30% pattern of eating-related cognitions and behaviours in an increase and 48% a decrease of appetite or intake, order to reduce or to maintain body weight (Herman & Table 1). Epidemiological data indicate that stress-related Mack, 1975). Another experiment demonstrated that eating is associated with increased body weight (Laitinen, emotional eaters (also identified by questionnaire; van Ek, & Sovio, 2002). Not surprisingly, effects of emotions Strien, Frijters, Bergers, & Defares, 1986) consume more on eating have been studied extensively, but due to their sweet, high-fat foods in response to emotional stress than variability it remains difficult to predict how an emotion non-emotional eaters (Oliver, Wardle & Gibson, 2000). affects eating in a given group of persons. Field studies showed that negative emotional states in Emotions can increase food intake in one group of everydaylifecanbeassociatedwithatendencytoeatasan persons, e.g. restrained eaters, but decrease food intake in emotion regulation strategy (Macht, Haupt, & Ellgring, another group, e.g. non-restrained eaters. Furthermore, 2005;Macht&Simons,2000).Emotionaleatingisthought different emotions may increase or decrease eating in the to be instigated in order to cope with negative emotions same group of individuals. For example, boredom may be (Bruch, 1973; Kaplan & Kaplan, 1957). associated with increased appetite, but sadness with Probably, the influences of both eating habits are decreased appetite (e.g. Pudel & Richter, 1980). This reflected in effects of emotions on binge eating. Negative twofold variability (across individuals and emotions) has emotionshavebeenfoundtoincreasethetendencytobinge been neglected by previous research. Based on an in bulimia nervosa and the binge eating disorder (experi- individual difference model, it was mostly examined ments: Agras & Telch, 1998; Cattanach, Malley, & Rodin, whether eating habits predispose people to increase food 1988; Chua, Touyz, & Hill, 2004; Gluck, Geliebter, Hung, intake in response to negative emotions (Greeno & Wing, & Yahav, 2004; Telch & Agras, 1996; field studies: Alpers 1994). The present paper introduces an integrative model &Tuschen-Caffier,2001;Cooper &Bowskill,1986;Davis, that takes into account both individual characteristics and Freeman, & Garner, 1988; Johnson & Larson, 1982; emotion features. First, the empirical evidence on varia- Kenardy, Arnow, & Agras, 1996). Taken together, studies indicate that restrained eaters increase food intake, emotional eaters consume more sweet, high-fat foods, Table1 and binge eaters tend to binge in response to negative Changesofeatinginresponsetoemotionalstressreportedinsurveys emotions. By contrast, normal eaters (i.e. normal-weight persons Changeofeating N Reference whose emotional and restrained eating scores fall within 11%moreappetite,70% 364 KrumbacherandMeyer the normal range) are usually assumed to reduce food lessappetite (1963) intakeinresponsetonegativeemotions(Herman&Polivy, 16%moreappetite,38% 1950 PudelandRichter(1980) 1984;Schachter,Goldman,&Gordon,1968).Theevidence lessappetite does not support this assumption. Table 2 summarizes 25%moreappetite,32% 1024 Pudel(1984) lessappetite experimentalstudiesinnormal-eatingpopulations:43%of 44%eatmore,48%eat 80 Willenbringetal.(1986) the results indicate an increase, 39% a decrease and 26% less no change of eating in response to emotions (as shown by 4%eatmore,55%eatless 475 Popperetal.(1989) Bellisle et al. (1990), in the latter studies possibly one part 49%eatmore,51donot 500 Spillman(1990) of the sample reduced and the other part increased food eatmore 55%eatmore,45%do 101 Weinsteinetal.(1997) intake). noteatmore Similarly, surveys and field studies showed that stress 38%eatmore,42%eat 212 OliverandWardle(1999) and negative emotions can be associated with both less increased and decreasedmotivation toeatandfoodintake ARTICLE IN PRESS M.Macht/Appetite50(2008)1–11 3 Table2 Effectsofemotionsoneating:experimentalstudiesinnormalsubjects Effect Eatingmeasure Emotion Reference Increase Foodintake Negativemood LoweandMaycock(1988) Negativemood Willneretal.(1998) Fear Pine(1985) Fear PinesandGal(1977) Fear Bellisleetal.(1990) Arousal Glass(1967) Arousal Cantoretal.(1982) Arousal GrunbergandStraub(1992) Boredom AbramsonandStinson(1977) Anger Macht(1996) Hedonicresponses Arousal/stress FerberandCabanac(1987) Joy Machtetal.(2002) Motivationtoeat Joy Machtetal.(2002) Anger Macht(1996) Chocolatecraving Negativemood Willneretal.(1998) Decrease Foodintake Negativemood BaucomandAiken(1981) Fear Schachteretal.(1968) Fear HermanandPolivy(1975) Fear Heathertonetal.(1991) Fear Hermanetal.(1987) Fear Bellisleetal.(1990) Arousal GrunbergandStraub(1992) Hedonicresponses Depressivemood WillnerandHealy(1994) Sadness Machtetal.(2002) Motivationtoeat Sadness Machtetal.(2002) Arousal Macht(1998) Chewing Arousal Macht(1998) Effect Eatingmeasure Emotion Reference Noeffect Foodintake Negativemood Frostetal.(1982) Negativemood Tuschenetal.(1993) Negativemood TelchandAgras(1996) Negativemood Ruderman(1985) Fear ReznickandBalch(1977) Fear Heathertonetal.(1991) Fear Schotteetal.(1990) Fear Coolsetal.(1992) (Table 1; de Castro, 1986; Hill, Weaver, & Blundell, 1991; Animal studies point to a role of intensity for the Macdiarmid & Hetherington, 1995; McCann, Warnick, & directionofeffectsofemotionalstressonfoodintake.Rats Knopp, 1990; Michaud et al., 1990; Pollard, Steptoe, decrease food intake during intense noise (Alario, Gama- Canaan,Davies, &Wardle,1995;Rosenfield&Stevenson, llo, Beato, & Trancho, 1987; Pare´, 1964), in response to 1988; Slochower, 1983; Steptoe, Lipsey, & Wardle, 1998; intense electric shock (Strongman, 1965; Weiss, 1968), and Stone & Brownell, 1994; Striegel-Moore et al., 1999; during chronic stress (Sampson, Muscat, Philips, & Will- Wardle, Steptoe, Oliver, & Lipsey, 2000; Weidner, Kohl- ner, 1992; Willner, Muscat, & Papp, 1992), but increase mann,Dotzauer,&Burns,1996).Basedonthisevidence,it foodintakeoreatingspeedinresponsetolowormoderate is difficult to predict how normal eaters change eating in noise and electric shock (Krebs et al., 1996; Kupferman, response to emotions. 1964; Rasbury & Shemberg, 1971; Sterritt, 1962, 1965; Sterritt & Shemberg, 1963; Strongman, Coles, Remington, Variability across emotions &Wookey,1970;Ullman,1951,1952).Resultsofahuman study correspond to these data (Mehrabian, 1980). Dimensions used to classify emotions, namely valence Participants imagined themselves in various emotions and (pleasure–displeasure), arousal (e.g. Greenwald, Cook, & reported how much they would eat during each of these Lang, 1989; Russell & Feldman Barrett, 1999) and emotions. Compared with low-arousal states (e.g. bore- intensity (Frijda, Ortony, Sonnemans, & Clore, 1992), dom, depressive mood) high-arousal states (e.g. tension, have been found to make an essential contribution to fear) were reported to inhibit food intake. These results variability of emotion-induced changes of eating. There is support the view that high-arousal or intense emotions clear evidence that emotions differing in valence and decrease food intake whereas low- to moderate emotions arousal or intensity exert different influences on eating. may increase food intake (Robbins & Fray, 1980). ARTICLE IN PRESS 4 M.Macht/Appetite50(2008)1–11 Table3 Previousmodelsofemotion-inducedchangesofeating Typeofmodel Explainedphenomenon Mediatingmechanism Scope Reference Psychological Increasedfoodintakeinresponseto Disinhibitionofrestrainedeating Restrainedeaters HermanandPolivy models emotions (1984) dto. Impairmentofcognitivecapacity Restrainedeaters Boonetal.(2002) Increasedfoodintakeinresponseto Reductionofnegativeemotionsby Emotionaleaters Bruch(1973), negativeemotions eating Slochower(1983), Booth(1994) Bingeeating Escapefromaversiveself-awareness Restrainedeaters, Heathertonand bulimics Baumeister(1991) Physiological Decreasedfoodintakeinresponse Antagonisticphysiologicalstress Animals,humans Schachteretal. models tostressandfear responses (1968) Decreasedhedonicreactionsduring Reducedbraindopamine Animals,humans Willneretal.(1992) chronicmildstress Eatinginducedbytail-pinch Activationofendogenousopiates Rats Morleyetal.(1983) Biologicalmodels Increasedeatingspeedinastressful Dispositiontoensureenergysupply Animals Lima(1987), environment Whishawetal. (1992) Eatingboutsinresponsetoarousal Reductionofarousalthrougheating Animal,humans Cantor(1981) andfrustration Human studies demonstrated that positive and negative Table 3, previous theories aim at different phenomena, emotions could differ markedly in their effects on eating. propose different mediating mechanisms and differ in Negative emotions such as anger, fear, and sadness may scope. It has been a major problem for research that increase impulsive eating (i.e. fast and irregular eating theories were mostly treated separately. Therefore, a directed at any food available), eating to regulate the general framework is provided in the following. It is emotional state and consumption of junk food, but suggested that emotions affect eating in five basic ways, decrease food pleasantness. By contrast, joy and other which encompass phenomena addressed by previous positive emotions increase food pleasantness and con- theories. sumption of healthy foods (Lyman, 1982; Macht, 1999; Macht, Roth, & Ellgring, 2002). A five-way model To summarize, high-arousal or intense emotions sup- press eating, and negative emotions can increase or Asshownabove,anumberoffactorswereidentifiedthat decrease food intake. Little is known about effects of makeanessentialcontributiontoemotion-inducedchanges positive emotions on eating and about differences between ofeating:arousal/intensity,valenceandfoodrelatednessof negative emotions such as anger, sadness and fear in their emotions as well as restrained and emotional eating. effects on eating. Clinical studies indicate that anger is an Taking into account these factors as antecedents, effects important antecedent of binge eating (Arnow, Kenardy, & of emotions on eating can be classified into five basic Agras, 1992). It has also been suggested that disgust classes: contributes to the maintenance of pathological eating patterns and may complicate attempts to normalize eating (1) Emotions aroused by food stimuli affect food choice. (Troop, Treasure, & Serpell, 2002). (2) Emotions high in arousal or intensity suppress eating due to incompatible emotional responses. Previous theories (3) Emotionsmoderateinarousalorintensityaffecteating depending on motivations to eat: During the past decades, a number of theories have (a) Inrestrainedeating,negativeandpositiveemotions attempted to explain the manifold effects of emotions on enhancefoodintakeduetoimpairmentofcognitive eating. Whereas emotion-induced increase of food intake control. was explained by psychological mechanisms (Bruch, 1969, (b) In emotional eating, negative emotions elicit the 1973; Heatherton & Baumeister, 1991; Herman & Polivy, tendency to be regulated by eating and, as a 1984; Slochower, 1983), physiological mechanisms were consequence, enhance intake of sweet and high-fat proposed to explain emotion-induced decrease of intake foods. (Herman & Polivy, 1984; Schachter et al., 1968; Willner, (c) In normal eating, emotions affect eating in con- 1997). Biologically oriented theories emphasized the gruence with their cognitive and motivational adaptive function of changes of feeding behaviour in features. stressful situations (Cantor, 1981; Lima, 1987; Whishaw, Dringenberg, & Comery, 1992). As can be seen from The flow diagram in Fig. 1 gives a visual presentation. ARTICLE IN PRESS M.Macht/Appetite50(2008)1–11 5 Emotion yes Preference Food-induced Control of Craving emotion? food choice Avoidance no yes Intense emotion? Suppression Decreased food intake no Impairment of Restrained cognitive eating Increased food intake eating controls yes Eating habits related to emotions? Eating to no Emotional regulate Increased intake of eating sweet, high-fat foods emotions yes Emotion features Congruent Change of eating congruent relevant to eating? modulation with emotion features no No change of eating Fig.1. Aflowdiagramtopredictbasicclassesofemotion-inducedchangesofeating. Emotionsoflowarousalorintensityarenotexpectedto Smulders, Havermans, & Jansen, 2004; Nederkoorn, affect eating. In the following, each class is described in Smulders, & Jansen, 2000) which vary across individuals detail. and can contribute to binge eating (Jansen, 1998). A number of studies demonstrated heightened cue reactivity Food-induced emotions control food choice inrestrainedeaters,bingeeatersandbulimicsascompared to controls (Carter, Bulik, McIntosh, & Joyce, 2001; Food-induced emotions are powerful determinants of Federoff, Polivy, & Herman, 2003; Karhunen, Lappalai- foodchoice(Galef,1996;Martins&Pliner,2005;Rozin& nen, Tammela, Turpeinen, & Uusitupa, 1997; Staiger, Schulkin, 1990). Tasting energy-dense food such as sugar Dawe, & McCarthy, 2000), although lack of differences and fat evokes positive affective responses that promote between groups has also been reported (e.g. Klaijner, ingestion, whereas tasting bitter compounds, which are Herman, Polivy, & Chhabra, 1980; Overduin, Jansen, & correlated with toxins, evokes negative affective responses Eilkes, 1997). It is important to recognize that in some that promote rejection (Rosenstein & Oster, 1988; Steiner, persons by contingent pairing of emotions and eating, 1979). The biological basis of these affective reactions is emotions may become food-related cues and thus elicit demonstratedinhomology acrossprimatespecies(Steiner, eating (Booth, 1994). Hawilo, Glaser, & Berridge, 2001). Notably, food-induced emotional responses encompass full-blown emotions, in Intense emotions suppress food intake particular, neophobia and disgust (Raudenbush & Frank, 1999; Rozin & Fallon, 1987). Animal studies demonstrated that intense or chronic Appetitive food-related stimuli can elicit a strong desire stresssharplydecreasesfoodintake(forreviewsseeGreeno to eat or craving that can be conceived as an additional &Wing,1994;Robbins&Fray,1980,Section‘‘Variability variant of food-induced emotions. Such craving co-occurs across emotions’’). Consequently, it was proposed that a with a number of autonomic responses (Nederkoorn, decreased food intake is the ‘‘natural’’ response to stress ARTICLE IN PRESS 6 M.Macht/Appetite50(2008)1–11 (Herman & Polivy, 1984; Schachter et al., 1968). In fact, Telch, 1998; Gluck et al., 2004) and can be treated intense emotions are linked with both behaviours and successfully by improving their skills to regulate emotions physiological responses that interfere with eating. For (Telch, Agras, & Linehan, 2001; Wiser & Telch, 1999). example, intense sadness is associated with behavioural Moreover, studies indicate that healthy, normal-weight deactivation and withdrawal from the environment. personsregulatenegativeemotionsbyeating(Macht,1999; Intense fear motivates flight and avoidance, and is also Macht et al., 2005; Macht & Simons, 2000). associatedwithautonomicresponsesinhibitingmotivation Emotionaleatingtheoryoriginatedfrompsychodynamic to eat. Stress-induced physiological changes may interfere thinking, but can be related to a number of more recent with digestion by delaying glucose absorption and gastro- approaches. The core assumption of the theory (i.e., that intestinal transit (Blair et al., 1991; Wing et al., 1990). negative emotions induce eating and are, as a result, reduced)hasbeenviewedfromalearningperspective,with Negative and positive emotions impair cognitive eating negative emotions as eliciting stimuli, eating as operant controls behaviour, and its consequence, the eating-induced reduc- tion of negative emotions, as negative reinforcement Restraint theory suggested that emotional stress disin- (Booth, 1994). Regulation was also taken into account by hibits dietary restraint and therefore increases food intake biological approaches which conceptualized arousal- (Herman & Polivy, 1984). In this view, negative emotions inducedeatingas‘‘adjunctivebehaviour’’or‘‘displacement were thought to undermine restrained eaters’ ability to activity’’ that reduces arousal (Cantor, 1981). Finally, continue dieting, because they impose a ‘‘more urgent eating has been viewed as a strategy to improve negative concern (i.e., how to cope with the stressor) than even mood (Thayer, 1989, 2001), to mask stress (masking dieting’’ and lead to eating ‘‘as if the diet boundary had hypothesis; Polivy & Herman, 1999), or to escape from been knocked down’’ (p. 152). The diet boundary was aversive self-awareness (escape theory; Heatherton & conceivedas‘‘thedieter’sself-imposedquotaforconsump- Baumeister, 1991). tiononagivenoccasion’’(p.149).Althoughthisappeared In spite of its popularity and plausibility, the concept of to be a plausible explanation, the common experimental emotional eating does however face some unresolved design comparing individuals with high or low scores on problems. Above all, the underlying mechanisms are far restraint scales was not appropriate to test putative from clear. Usually, nutrient-dependent physiological mediating mechanisms (Greeno & Wing, 1994). changes are proposed to mediate mood-elevating effects An alternative explanation was suggested by Boon, of eating. Carbohydrate-rich meals may lead to a Stroebe, Schut, and Jansen (1998). According to their postprandial improvement of mood (for reviews see ‘‘limited capacity hypothesis’’, restrained eaters’ food Benton&Donohoe,1999;Christensen,1993,1997;Gibson intake is increased, if their cognitive capacity to maintain & Green, 2002) and also to reduced stress responses in restricted food intake is limited by distraction (be it emotionallylabilesubjects(Markus,Panhuysen,&Tuiten, emotionalornot).Processingofemotionalstimulirequires 1998). These effects were explained by meal-induced attention, and since cognitive capacity is limited, cognitive increase of brain serotonin availability (Wurtman, 1982). control over eating may be impaired. The overeating However, relatively small proportions of protein in a meal response was attributed to ironic processes, i.e., the more may deteriorate the increase of tryptophan levels at the peopleexertcognitivecontrol,thehighertheirvulnerability blood–brain barrier and, thus, an increase of brain to overeating will be. A number of experimental studies serotonin. Since most carbohydrate-rich mealsin everyday supported this assumption (Boon, Stroebe, Schut, & life contain some protein, the ecological validity of the IJntema, 2002; Boon et al., 1998; Lattimore & Caswell, serotonin hypothesis can be questioned (Benton, 2002). 2004;Lattimore&Maxwell,2004;Vreugdenberg,Bryan,& Another physiological mechanism is suggested by animal Kemps, 2003; Wallis & Hetherington, 2004; Ward & data demonstrating that ingestion of high fat and Mann, 2000). carbohydrate ‘‘comfort foods’’ (Wansink, Cheney, & Chan,2003)reducesactivityofthehypothalamo-pituitary– Negative emotions elicit eating to regulate emotions adrenal axis and dampens stress responses (Dallman et al., 2003). Emotional eating theory suggested that certain indivi- The main problem with both the serotonin and the duals eat in order to cope with stress and are therefore at endocrinehypothesisisthatemotionaleffectsofeatingcan risk of becoming obese (Bruch, 1969, 1973; Kaplan, 1957; occuronlywithdelay.Suchdelayedeffectsmightbeuseful Slochower, 1983). As the German colloquialism ‘‘Kum- for coping with chronic stress, but not for responding to merspeck’’ (literally: fat of sorrow) shows, it is a popular negativeemotions,astheyarisefromunpredictablestimuli view that emotional eating causes obesity. Although this suchasdailyhassles.Inordertocopewiththeseemotions, view proved to be over-simplistic (Greeno & Wing, 1994), it would be most efficient to reduce their intensity evidence did emerge for a role of emotional eating in a immediately. Indeed, sweet solutions have been shown to subgroup of obese persons. Obese binge eaters experience rapidly calm stress responses in human newborns (Smith, anincreasedtendencytobingeinnegativemood(Agras& Fillion, &Blass, 1990). Similarly,in adults, experimentally ARTICLE IN PRESS M.Macht/Appetite50(2008)1–11 7 induced negative mood is improved immediately and such as thirst, sex and aggression. To improve under- selectively after eating palatable food, and this effect is standing of the manifold effects of emotions on eating most pronounced in persons scoring high on emotional research has to disentangle mediating mechanisms and to eating (Macht & Mu¨ller, 2007). This suggests that identify relations between classes of emotion-induced immediate positive affective reactions elicited by palatable changes of eating. In particular, two problems remain to foodsdiminishtheimpactofstress.Additionally,itmaybe be resolved. important here that eatingdistracts from the experience of negative emotions (Spitzer & Rodin, 1983). One might Emotion-induced eating in restrained and emotional eaters speculate that immediate effects of eating based on hedonistic mechanisms and distraction, play a major role Negative emotions increase food intake in restrained in the regulation of everyday emotions and constitute a eatersaswellasintakeofsweet,high-fatfoodinemotional common phenomenon that applies to the general popula- eaters (Section ‘‘Variability across individual’’). To tion. By contrast, in binge eating, immediate hedonic explain these findings, it was suggested that emotional effects may be backed up or substituted by delayed and eaters eat to reduce negative emotions. By contrast, long-lasting physiological effects of chronic high fat and restrained eaters were thought to increase food intake, carbohydrate consumption. because emotions impair cognitive control over their restricted eating pattern (Sections ‘‘Negative and positive Emotions modulate eating in congruence with emotion emotions impair cognitive eating controls’’ and ‘‘Negative features emotions elicit eating to regulate emotions’’). Although restrained and emotional eating appear to reflect different During negative mood, negative verbal information is neurobiological processes (Volkow et al., 2003), it remains retrieved more readily than positive information, and unclear whether increased consumption in response to conversely, positive information is retrieved more readily emotions is in fact mediated by different mechanisms in during positive mood (for a review, see Berkowitz, 2000, restrained and emotional eaters. The present model pp. 78–83). This congruency effect was also shown for suggests two ways to test empirically the assumption of recognition of palatable and unpalatable food flavours different mechanisms. (Pliner&Steverango,1994)aswellasforemotion-induced First, emotional and restrained eaters should differ changesofeating.Sadnesshasbeenfoundtodecreaseand intheireatingresponsestovariousemotionsandstressors. joy to increase food pleasantness and motivation to eat Provided that cognitive control of eating can be imp- (Machtetal.,2002;Willner&Healy,1994).Thesechanges aired by a variety of influences, restrained eaters should canbeexplainedbyemotionfeatures.Sadnessisassociated increase food intake in response to both negative and with a slowing of cognitive processes and motor activity positive emotions and also to cognitive demand. (Izard & Ackerman, 2000), with an absence of interest By contrast, emotional eaters should increase eating only (Frijda,1986,p.22),andwithaloweringofattentiontothe in response to negative emotions. Second, emotion- outside(Barr-Zisowitz,2000).Bycontrast,joyisassociated induced eating should exert different effects on emotional withanincreasedcapabilitytoperceiveandprocessstimuli state in emotional and restrained eaters. Consistent and an increased readiness to engage in activities (Freder- with the assumption that emotional eaters increase food ickson,1998;Frijda,1986;Izard&Ackerman,2000).Such intaketoreducenegativeemotions,theycanbeexpectedto emotion-congruentchangesofeatingcanbeconceivedasa report reduced negative emotions due to eating. by-product of emotional activation, and should mostly By contrast, restrained eaters should experience no change occur, if emotions are elicited during eating. of mood due to emotion-induced eating or may even report deterioration of mood (because of fear to gain Discussion weight). There is some evidence supporting these predictions. Thepresentpaperproposesfivebasicclassesofemotion- Restrainedeatershaveindeedbeenshowntoincreasefood induced changes of eating that can be predicted by intake not only in response to negative emotions, but also antecedent conditions and point to basic functional to positive emotions (Cools, Schotte, & McNnally, 1992; principles relating emotions and eating. Depending on Tuschen et al., 1993) and cognitive demand (e.g. Wallis & emotion features and eating habits emotion-induced Hetherington, 2004); it remains, however, to be examined changes of eating can be a result of interference (suppres- whether emotional eaters’ intake can also be increased by sion of food intake; impairment of cognitive eating positive emotions and non-emotional stressors. Also, controls), a by-product of emotions (emotion-congruent improvement of experimentally induced negative mood modulation of eating), and a consequence of regulatory by eating has been found to be correlated with emotional, processes (emotional control of food choice; eating to but not with restrained eating (Macht & Mu¨ller, 2007). In regulate emotions). Future research may examine whether spite of this evidence, however, more studies comparing these principles can be applied to relations between emotion-induced changes of eating in restrained and emotions and other biologically motivated behaviours emotional eaters are needed. ARTICLE IN PRESS 8 M.Macht/Appetite50(2008)1–11 Emotion-induced eating in normal eaters References Innormaleaters,negativeemotionsdecreaseorincrease Abramson,E.E.,&Stinson,S.G.(1977).Boredomandeatinginobese foodintake.Thefive-waymodelexplainsdecreasedintake, andnon-obeseindividuals.AddictiveBehaviors,2,181–185. Agras,W.S.,&Telch,C.F.(1998).Theeffectsofcaloricdeprivationand depending on emotion intensity, by emotion-congruent negative affect on binge eating in obese binge-eating disordered modulation (Section ‘‘Intense emotions suppress food women.BehaviorTherapy,29,491–503. intake) or emotional suppression of eating (Section Alario,P.,Gamallo,A.,Beato,M.J.,&Trancho,G.(1987).Bodyweight ‘‘Emotions modulate eating in congruence with emotion gain, food intake and adrenal development in chronic noise stressed features’’), butitremainsunclear howtoexplainincreased rats.Physiology&Behavior,40,29–32. Alpers, G. W., & Tuschen-Caffier, B. (2001). Negative feelings and the intake in normal eaters. To understand this phenomenon, desiretoeatinbulimianervosa.EatingBehaviors,2,339–352. it should be recognized that emotional and restrained Arnow,B.,Kenardy,J.,&Agras,W.S.(1992).Bingeeatingamongthe eating are regarded as continuous variables and that obese: A descriptive study. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 15, persons are classified as emotional or restrained eaters, if 155–170. theyscorehighonthesecontinua.Forexample,emotional Barr-Zisowitz,C.(2000).‘‘Sadness’’—Istheresuchathing?InM.L.J.M. eaters may be defined as lying above the 80th percentile of Haviland (Ed.), Handbook of emotions (2nd ed., pp. 607–622). New York:GuilfordPress. scores in the most appropriate norm group (van Strien, Baucom, D. H., & Aiken, P. A. (1981). Effect of depressed mood on 2002; personal communication). These persons can be eating: Among obese and nonobese dieting and nondieting persons. expected to eat in order to regulate negative emotions JournalofPersonalityandSocialPsychology,41,577–585. frequently and consistently across situations. Presumably, Bekker, M. H. J., van de Meerendonk, C., & Mollerus, J. (2004). ahighproportionoftheirmealsandsnacksisbasedonthe Effectsofnegativemoodinductionandimpulsivityonself-perceived emotional eating. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 36, motivation to eat in order to cope with negative emotions. 461–469. Persons with low scores of the emotional eating scale (e.g. Bellisle, F., Louis-Sylvestre, J., Linet, N., Rocaboy, B., Dalle, B., below the 50th percentile) can be expected to experience Cheneau, F., et al. (1990). Anxiety and food intake in man. this motivation, if at all, less frequently and less consis- PsychosomaticMedicine,52(4),452–457. tently. However, normal eaters with moderate emotional Benton, D. (2002). Carbohydrate ingestion, blood glucose and mood. eating scores (e.g. between the 60th and 80th percentile) NeuroscienceandBiobehavioralReview,26,293–308. Benton,E.,&Donohoe,R.T.(1999).Theeffectsofnutrientsonmood. may occasionally also eat to cope with negative emotions. PublicHealthNutrition,2,403–409. Inthesepersons,atransienttendencytoregulateemotions Berkowitz, L. (2000). Causes and consequences of feelings. Cambridge: by eating may enhance consumption during emotions, CambridgeUniversityPress. although they are not classified as emotional eaters. Blair, E. H., Wing, R. R., & Wald, A. (1991). The effect of laboratory It has indeed been demonstrated that normal eaters stressors on glycemic control and gastrointestinal transit time. PsychosomaticMedicine,53,133–143. increase motivation to regulate emotions by eating in Boon,B.,Stroebe,W.,Schut,H.,&IJntema,R.(2002).Ironicprocessesin response to negative emotions induced in the laboratory the eating behaviour of restrained eaters. British Journal of Health (Bekker, van de Meerendonk, & Mollerus, 2004) and Psychology,7,1–10. experienced in everyday life (Macht et al., 2005; Macht & Boon,B.,Stroebe,W.,Schut,H.,&Jansen,A.(1998).Foodforthought: Simons, 2000). Similarly, normal eaters may occasionally Cognitive regulation of food intake. British Journal of Health restrict eating, although they do not score very high on Psychology,3,27–40. Booth,D.A.(1994).Psychologyofnutrition.London:Taylor&Francis. restraint scales, and thus enhance their susceptibility to Bruch, H. (1969). Hunger and instinct. Journal of Nervous and Mental emotion-induced eating. It can therefore be assumed that Disease,149,91–114. normal eaters increase intake due to transient shifts in Bruch, H. (1973). Eating disorders. Obesity and anorexia nervosa. motivationstoeat.Inotherwords,impairmentofcognitive Riverside:BehaviorScienceBookService. eating controls and emotion regulation eating (Sections Cantor,M.B.(1981).Badhabits:Modelsofinducedingestioninsatiated rats and people. In S. Miller (Ed.), Behavior and nutrition. Franklin ‘‘Negative and positive emotions impair cognitive eating InstitutePress. controls’’ and Negative emotions elicit eating to regulate Cantor,M.B.,Smith,S.E.,&Bryan,B.R.(1982).Inducedbadhabits: emotions) may occasionally also occur in normal eaters. Adjunctive ingestion and grooming in human subjects. Appetite, 3, Future research is needed to test the hypothesis that 1–12. transientshiftsinmotivationstoeatenhancesusceptibility Carter, F. A., Bulik, C. M., McIntosh, V. V., & Joyce, P. R. (2001). innormaleaterstoincreaseintakeinresponsetoemotions. Changes in cue reactivity following treatment for bulimia nervosa. InternationalJournalofEatingDisorders,29,336–344. A suitable starting point for an experimental approach Cattanach, L., Malley, R., & Rodin, J. (1988). Psychologic and could be a study by Willner et al. (1998), who reinforced physiologic reactivity to stressors in eating disordered individuals. responses with chocolate under a progressively increasing PsychosomaticMedicine,50,591–599. work requirement and showed that craving for and intake Christensen,L.(1993).Effectsofeatingbehavioronmood:Areviewof ofchocolatewereincreasedbydepressivemood.Itremains theliterature.InternationalJournalofEatingDisorders,14,171–183. Christensen,L.(1997).Theeffectofcarbohydratesonaffect.Nutrition,13, to be examined whether the linking of operant responding 503–514. for food and depressive mood can transiently increase Chua, J. L., Touyz, S., & Hill, A. J. (2004). Negative mood-induced motivationtoeatinordertoregulatemoodinpersonsnot overeatinginobesebingeeaters:Anexperimentalstudy.International classified as typical emotional eaters. JournalofObesity,28,606–610. ARTICLE IN PRESS M.Macht/Appetite50(2008)1–11 9 Cools, J., Schotte, D. E., & McNnally, R. J. (1992). Emotional arousal Herman, C. P., & Mack, D. (1975). Restraint and unrestraint eating. andovereatinginrestrainedeaters.JournalofAbnormalPsychology, JournalofPersonality,43,647–660. 101,348–351. Herman,C.P.,&Polivy,J.(1975).Anxiety,restraintandeatingbehavior. Cooper, P. J., & Bowskill, R. (1986). Dysphoric mood and overeating. JournalofAbnormalPsychology,84,662–672. BritishJournalofClinicalPsychology,25,155–156. Herman,C.P.,&Polivy,J.(1984).Aboundarymodelfortheregulation Dallman,M.F.,Pecoraro,N.,Akana,S.F.,laFleur,S.E.,Gomez,F., ofeating.EatingandItsDisorders,141–156. Houshyar,H.,etal.(2003).Chronicstressandobesity:Anewviewof Herman, C. P., Polivy, J., Lank, C. N., & Heatherton, T. F. (1987). ‘‘comfortfood’’.ProceedingsoftheNationalAcademyofSciences,100, Anxiety, hunger, and eating behavior. Journal of Abnormal Psychol- 11696–11701. ogy,96,264–269. Davis, R., Freeman, R. J., & Garner, D. M. (1988). A naturalistic Hill, A. J., Weaver, C. F. L., & Blundell, J. E. (1991). Food craving, investigation of eating behavior in bulimia nervosa. Journal of dietaryrestraintandmood.Appetite,17,187–197. ConsultingandClinicalPsychology,56,273–279. Izard,C.E.,&Ackerman,B.P.(2000).Organizationalandmotivational DeCastro,J.M.(1986).Macronutrientrelationshipswithmealpatterns functions of discrete emotions. In M. L. J. M. Haviland (Ed.), andmoodinthespontaneousfeedingbehaviorofhumans.Physiology Handbook of emotions (2nd ed., pp. 253–264). New York: Guilford &Behavior,39,561–569. Press. Ekman,P.(1992).Anargumentforbasicemotions.Cognition&Emotion, Jansen,A.(1998).Alearningmodelofbingeeating:Cuereactivityandcue 6,169–200. exposure.BehaviourResearchandTherapy,36,257–272. Federoff, I., Polivy, J., & Herman, C. P. (2003). The specificity of Johnson, C., & Larson, R. (1982). Bulimia: An analysis of mood and restrained vs. unrestrained eaters’ responses to food cues: General behavior.PsychosomaticMedicine,44(4),341–351. desiretoeat,orcravingforthecuedfood?Appetite,41,7–13. Kaplan, H. I., & Kaplan, H. S. (1957). The psychosomatic concept of Ferber, C., & Cabanac, M. (1987). Influence of noise on gustatory obesity.JournalofNervousandMentalDisease,125,181–201. affective ratings and preference for sweet and salt. Appetite, 8, Karhunen,L.J.,Lappalainen,R.I.,Tammela, L.,Turpeinen,A.K.,& 229–235. Uusitupa,M.I.J.(1997).Subjectiveandphysiologicalcephalicphase Frederickson, B. (1998). What good are positive emotions? Review of responsestofoodinobesebinge-eatingwomen.InternationalJournal GeneralPsychology,2,300–319. ofEatingDisorders,21,321–328. Frijda,N.(1986).Theemotions.Cambridge:CambridgeUniversityPress. Kenardy, J., Arnow, B., & Agras, W. S. (1996). The aversiveness of Frijda,N.(1993).Moods,emotionepisodes,andemotions.InM.L.J.M. specificemotionalstatesassociatedwithbinge-eatinginobesesubjects. Haviland (Ed.), Handbook of emotions (pp. 381–403). London, New AustralianandNewZealandJournalofPsychiatry,30(6),839–844. York:GuilfordPress. Klaijner, F., Herman, P. C., Polivy, J., & Chhabra, R. (1980). Human Frijda, N., Ortony, A., Sonnemans, J., & Clore, G. L. (1992). The obesity, dieting, and anticipatory salivation to food. Physiology & complexity of intensity—Issues concerning the structure of emotion Behavior,27,195–198. intensity.InM.S.Clark(Ed.),Emotion(pp.60–89).NewburyPark: Krebs, H., Macht, M., Weyers, P., Weijers, H.-G., & Janke,W. (1996). SagePublications. Effects of stressful noise on eating and non-eating behavior in rats. Frost,R.O.,Goolkasian, G.A.,Ely,R.J., &Blanchard,F. A.(1982). Appetite,26,193–202. Depression, restraint and eating behavior. Behavioral Research and Krumbacher, K., & Meyer, J. E. (1963). Das Appetitverhalten des Therapy,20,113–121. gesunden unter emotionalem StreX. Psychosomatische Medizin, 9, Galef, J. R. (1996). Food selection: Problems in understanding how we 89–94. choose food to eat. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 20(1), Kupferman, I. (1964). Eating behavior induced by sounds. Nature, 201, 67–73. 324. Gibson, E. L. (2006). Emotional influences on food choice: Sensory, Laitinen,J.,Ek,E.,&Sovio,U.(2002).Stress-relatedeatinganddrinking physiologicalandpsychologicalpathways.Physiology&Behavior,89, behavior and body mass index and predictors of this behavior. 53–61. PreventiveMedicine,34,29–39. Gibson,E.L.,&Green,M.W.(2002).Nutritionalinfluencesoncognitive Lattimore, P., & Caswell, N. (2004). Differential effects of active and function: Mechanisms of susceptibility. Nutrition Research Reviews, passive stress on food intake in restrained and unrestrained eaters. 15,169–206. Appetite,42,167–173. Glass, L. B. (1967). The generality of oral-consummatory behavior of Lattimore, P., & Maxwell, L. (2004). Cognitive load, stress, and alcoholicsunderstress.DissertationAbstractsInternational,28,5205. disinhibitedeating.EatingBehaviors,5,315–324. Gluck, M. E., Geliebter, A., Hung, J., & Yahav, E. (2004). Cortisol, Lima,S.L.(1987).Vigilancewhilefeedinganditsrelationtotheriskof hunger, and desire to binge eat following a cold stress test in obese predation.JournalofTheoreticalBiology,124,303–316. women with binge eating disorder. Psychosomatic Medicine, 66, Lowe,M.R.,&Maycock,B.(1988).Restraint,disinhibition,hungerand 876–881. negativeaffecteating.AddictiveBehaviors,13,369–377. Greeno,G.G.,&Wing,R.R.(1994).Stress-inducedeating.Psychological Lyman,B.(1982).Thenutritionalvaluesandfoodgroupcharacteristicsof Bulletin,115,444–464. foodspreferredduringvariousemotions.JournalofPsychology,112, Greenwald,M.K.,Cook,E.W.,&Lang,P.J.(1989).Affectivejudgement 121–127. and psychophysiological response: Dimensional covariation in the Macdiarmid,J.I.,&Hetherington,M.M.(1995).Moodmodulationby evaluation of pictorial stimuli. Journal of Psychophysiology, 3, food: An exploration of affect and cravings in ‘‘chocolate addicts’’. 51–64. BritishJournalofClinicalPsychology,34,129–138. Grunberg, N. E., & Straub, R. O. (1992). The role of gender and taste Macht, M. (1996). Effects of high- and low-energy meals on hunger, class in the effects of stress on eating. Health Psychology, 11(2), physiologicalprocessesandreactionstoemotionalstress.Appetite,26, 97–100. 71–88. Heatherton, T. F., & Baumeister, R. F. (1991). Binge eating as escape Macht,M.(1998).Effectsofnoise-inducedarousalonchewingofsweet fromself-awareness.PsychologicalBulletin,110,86–108. foodandthesubjectivemotivationtoeat.NutritionalNeuroscience,1, Heatherton,T.F.,Herman,C.P.,&Polivy,J.(1991).Effectsofphysical 213–222. threat and ego threat on eating behavior. Journal of Personality and Macht, M. (1999). Characteristics of eating in anger, fear, sadness, and SocialPsychology,60,138–143. joy.Appetite,33,129–139. Heatherton, T. F., Striepe, M., & Wittenberg, L. (1998). Emotional Macht,M.,Haupt,C.,&Ellgring,H.(2005).Theperceivedfunctionof distressanddisinhibitedeating:Theroleofself.PersonalityandSocial eating is changed during examination stress: A field study. Eating PsychologyBulletin,24(3),301–313. Behaviors,6,109–112. ARTICLE IN PRESS 10 M.Macht/Appetite50(2008)1–11 Macht, M., & Mu¨ller, J. (2007). Immediate effects of chocolate on Rasbury, W., & Shemberg, K. (1971). The effects of aversive levels of experimentally induced mood states. Appetite, doi:10.1016/j. white noise on consummatory behavior. Psychonomic Science, 22, appet.2007.05.004. 166–167. Macht,M.,Roth,S.,&Ellgring,H.(2002).Chocolateeatinginhealthy Raudenbush,B.,&Frank,R.A.(1999).Assessingfoodneophobia:The men during experimentally induced sadness and joy. Appetite, 39, roleofstimulusfamiliarity.Appetite,32,261–271. 147–158. Reznick,H.,&Balch,P.(1977).Theeffectsofanxietyandresponsecost Macht,M., & Simons,G. (2000). Emotions and eatingin everydaylife. manipulations on the eating behavior of obese and normal weight Appetite,35,65–71. subjects.AddictiveBehaviors,2,219–225. Markus,C.R.,Panhuysen,G.,&Tuiten,A.(1998).Doescarbohydrate- Robbins,T.W.,&Fray,P.J.(1980).Stress-inducedeating:Fact,fiction rich,protein-poorfoodpreventadeteriorationofmoodandcognitive ormisunderstanding?Appetite,1,103–133. performanceofstress-pronesubjectswhensubjectedtoastressfultask. Rosenfield,S.N.,&Stevenson,J.S.(1988).Perceptionofdailystressand Appetite,31,49–65. oralcopingbehaviorsinnormal,overweight,andrecoveringalcoholic Martins,Y.,&Pliner,P.(2005).Humanfoodchoices:Anexaminationof women.ResearchinNursing&Health,11,165–174. the factors underlying acceptance/rejection of novel and familiar Rosenstein, D., & Oster, H. (1988). Differential facial responsesto four animalandnonanimalfoods.Appetite,45,214–224. basictastesinnewborns.ChildDevelopment,59,1555–1568. McCann, B. S., Warnick, G. R., & Knopp, R. H. (1990). Changes in Rotenberg, K. J., & Flood, D. (1999). Loneliness, dysphoria, dietary plasma lipids and dietary intake accompanying shifts in perceived restraint, and eating behavior. International Journal of Eating workloadandstress.PsychosomaticMedicine,52,97–108. Disorders,25(1),55–64. Mehrabian,A.(1980).Basicdimensionsforageneralpsychologicaltheory. Rozin,P.,&Fallon,A.E.(1987).Aperspectiveondisgust.Psychological Cambridge:Oelgeschlager,Gunn&Hain. Review,94(1),23–41. Michaud,C.,Kahn,J.P.,Musse,N.,Burlet,C.,Nicolas,J.P.,&Mejean, Rozin,P.,&Schulkin,J.(1990).Foodselection.InE.M.Stricker(Ed.), L. (1990). Relationships between a critical life event and eating Neurobiology of food and fluid intake (pp. 297–328). New York: behaviourinhigh-schoolstudents.StressMedicine,6,57–64. PlenumPress. Morley, J. E., Levine, A. S., & Rowland, N. E. (1983). Stress-induced Ruderman, A. J. (1985). Dysphoric mood and overeating: A test of eating.LifeSciences,32,2169–2182. restraint theory’s disinhibition hypothesis. Journal of Abnormal Nederkoorn, C., Smulders, F., Havermans, R., & Jansen, A. (2004). Psychology,94,78–85. Exposuretobingefoodinbulimianervosa:Fingerpulseamplitudeas Russell, J. A., & Feldman Barrett, L. (1999). Core affect, prototypical a potential measure of urge to eat and predictor of food intake. emotional episodes, and other things called emotion: Dissecting the Appetite,42,125–130. elephant.JournalofPersonalityandSocialPsychology,76,805–819. Nederkoorn,C.,Smulders,F.T.Y.,&Jansen,A.(2000).Cephalicphase Rutledge, T., & Linden, W. (1998). To eat or not to eat: Affective and responses, craving and food intake in normal subjects. Appetite, 35, physiologicalmechanismsinthestress-eatingrelationship.Journalof 45–55. BehavioralMedicine,21,221–240. Oliver,G.,&Wardle,J.(1999).Perceivedeffectsofstressonfoodchoice. Sampson, D., Muscat, R., Philips, G., & Willner, P. (1992). Decreased Physiology&Behavior,66,511–515. reactivitytosweetnessfollowingchronicexposuretomildunpredict- Oliver, G., Wardle, J., & Gibson, E. L. (2000). Stress and food choice: able stress or acute administration of pimozide. Neuroscience and Alaboratorystudy.PsychosomaticMedicine,62,853–865. BiobehavioralReviews,16(4),519–524. Overduin,J.,Jansen,A.,&Eilkes,H.(1997).Cuereactivitytofood-and Schachter, S., Goldman,R., &Gordon,A. (1968).Effectsof fear, food body-related stimuli in restrained and unrestrained eaters. Addictive deprivation,and obesity on eating. Journalof Personalityand Social Behaviors,22,395–404. Psychology,10,91–97. Pare´,W.P.(1964).Theeffectofchronicenvironmentalstressonstomach Schotte,D.E.,Cools,J.,&McNally,R.J.(1990).Film-inducednegative ulceration, adrenal function,and consummatory behavior in the rat. affect triggers overeating in restrained eaters. Journal of Abnormal JournalofPsychology,57,143–151. Psychology,99,317–320. Pine, C. J. (1985). Anxiety and eating behavior in obese and nonobese Slochower, J. (1983). Excessive eating: The role of emotions and American Indians and white Americans. Journal of Personality and environment.NewYork:HumanSciencesPress. SocialPsychology,49,774–780. Smith,B.A.,Fillion,T.J.,&Blass,E.M.(1990).Orallymediatedsources Pines,C.J.,&Gal,R.(1977).Theeffectoffoodontestanxiety.Journalof of calming in 1- to 3-day-old human infants. Developmental AppliedSocialPsychology,49,774–780. Psychology,26(5),731–737. Pliner,P.,&Steverango,C.(1994).Effectofinducedmoodonmemory Spillman,D.(1990).Surveyoffoodandvitaminintakeresponsesreported forflavors.Appetite,22,135–148. by university students experiencing stress. Psychological Reports, 66, Plutchik, R. (1984). Emotions: A general psychoevolutionary theory. In 499–502. K. R. Scherer (Ed.), Approaches toemotion (pp. 197–219).Hillsdale: Spitzer, L., & Rodin, J. (1983). Arousal-induced eating: Conventional LawrenceErlbaum. wisdomorempiricalfindings?InJ.Capiocco,&R.Petty(Eds.),Social Polivy,J.,&Herman,C.P.(1999).Distressandeating:Whydodieters psychophysiology(pp.565–591).NewYork:GuilfordPress. overeat?InternationalJournalofEatingDisorders,26,153–164. Staiger,P.,Dawe,S.,&McCarthy,R.(2000).Responsivitytofoodcuesin Pollard,T.M.,Steptoe,A.,Canaan,L.,Davies,G.J.,&Wardle,J.(1995). bulimicwomenandcontrols.Appetite,35,27–33. Effects of academic examination stress on eating behavior and Steiner, J. E. (1979). Human facial expressions in response to taste and blood lipid levels. International Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 2(4), smell stimulation. Advances in Child Development and Behavior, 13, 299–320. 257–295. Popper,R.,Smits,G.,Meiselman,H.L.,&Hirsch,E.(1989).Eatingin Steiner, J. E., Glaser, D., Hawilo, M. E., & Berridge, K. C. (2001). combat:AsurveyofUSMarines.MilitaryMedicine,154,619–623. Comparative expression of hedonic impact: Affective reactions to Pudel,V.(1984).PsychosozialeBewertungderErna¨hrunginFamilienmit tastesandotherprimates.NeuroscienceandBiobehavioralReviews,25, Kindern.EineRepra¨sentativerhebunginderBundesrepublikDeutsch- 53–74. land.InDeutscheGesellschaftfu¨rErna¨hrung(Ed.),Erna¨hrungsbericht Steptoe,A.,Lipsey,Z.,&Wardle,J.(1998).Stress,hasslesandvariations (pp.103–144).FrankfurtamMain:Henrich. in alcohol consumption, food choice and physical exercise. British Pudel,V.,&Richter,M.(1980).Psychosozialebewertungdererna¨hrung. JournalofHealthPsychology,3,51–63. Einerepra¨sentativ-erhebunginderbundesrepublikdeutschland.InD. Sterritt, G. M. (1962). Inhibition and faciliation of eating by electric G. f. Erna¨hrung (Ed.), Erna¨hrungsbericht. Frankfurt: Deutsche shock. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 55, Gesellschaftfu¨rErna¨hrung. 226–229.